2004
DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.813
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Biases in Social Comparative Judgments: The Role of Nonmotivated Factors in Above-Average and Comparative-Optimism Effects.

Abstract: Biases in social comparative judgments, such as those illustrated by above-average and comparative-optimism effects, are often regarded as products of motivated reasoning (e.g., self-enhancement). These effects, however, can also be produced by information-processing limitations or aspects of judgment processes that are not necessarily biased by motivational factors. In this article, the authors briefly review motivational accounts of biased comparative judgments, introduce a 3-stage model for understanding ho… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

29
485
2
10

Year Published

2007
2007
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 477 publications
(543 citation statements)
references
References 134 publications
(332 reference statements)
29
485
2
10
Order By: Relevance
“…Hertzog & Hultsch, 2000), or follow a self-serving bias by expecting themselves to be above-average (cf. Chambers & Windschitl, 2004), as indicated by the middlepoint of the scale. If the objective difficulty of the PM task is then quite high and thus the PM performance rather low, as in our first experiment, these ill-calibrated performance-predictions are likely to result in an overestimation of the actual PM-performance.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Hertzog & Hultsch, 2000), or follow a self-serving bias by expecting themselves to be above-average (cf. Chambers & Windschitl, 2004), as indicated by the middlepoint of the scale. If the objective difficulty of the PM task is then quite high and thus the PM performance rather low, as in our first experiment, these ill-calibrated performance-predictions are likely to result in an overestimation of the actual PM-performance.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The first alternative theoretical account suggests that the desirability bias evidenced in studies that examine optimism about the likelihood of future life events is the result of nonmotivational factors (see Chambers & Windschitl, 2004, for a review), and thus affective reactions are unlikely to be the cause of the desirability bias in judgments about future life events. According to these nonmotivational accounts, the desirability bias results from methodological factors in the studies themselves and biases in how people consider others while making judgments.…”
Section: Related Theoretical Accountsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The lack of studies that manipulate the desirability of future events has meant that there is no evidence that desire creates biases in judgments about real life events. Further, there is empirical evidence that methodological factors and psychological factors related to the way people process comparative questions can alone create undue optimism or pessimism in judgments (e.g., Chambers & Windschitl, 2004;Kruger & Burrus, 2004). Study 1 thus manipulated positive, negative, and neutral affective reactions to an initially neutral potential future life event, and participants judged their chances relative to a peer's.…”
Section: Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, social networking sites provide opportunities for self-presentation (Manago et al 2008) by offering a gateway via favourable self-descriptions and photos (Buffardi and Cambell 2008). People engage in self-enhancing concerns (Chambers and Windschitl 2004;Schlosser 2011) to give others a positive impression about themselves (Vohs, Baumeister and Ciarocco 2005). People generate positive WOM of their own experiences due to self-enhancement motives (De Angelis et al 2012).…”
Section: Self-enhancementmentioning
confidence: 99%