2014
DOI: 10.1177/1541931214581458
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Auditory and Visual Peripheral Detection Tasks and the Lane Change Test with High and Low Cognitive Load

Abstract: The current study measured driving performance using the Lane Change Test (LCT) and a Peripheral Detection Task (PDT), to determine if stimulus modality (visual and auditory) and stimulus eccentricity would affect performance on the PDT. The peripheral stimuli were either small white circles or broadband noise bursts located either 13o or 19o to the left or right of the center lane. Participants were instructed to respond with left or right key presses to the stimuli’s relative location while performing the LC… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
2

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 5 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, according to the cognitive control hypothesis, the DRT should be sensitive to interference from cognitively loading secondary tasks. This prediction is confirmed by a large number of studies reporting that cognitively loading tasks increase DRT response times relative to a baseline (no-task) condition, with effects typically in the range of 100 to 300 ms (Bruyas & Dumont, 2013; Chong et al, 2014; Conti, Dlugosch, Vilimek, Keinath, & Bengler, 2012; Diels, 2011; Engström, Åberg, Johansson, & Hammarbäck, 2005; Engström, Larsson, & Larsson, 2013; Harbluk, Burns, Hernandez, Tam, & Glazduri, 2013; Mantzke & Keinath, 2015; Merat & Jamson, 2008; Merat, Kountouriotis, Tomlinson, Carsten, & Engström, 2015; Nilsson et al, 2017; Patten, Kircher, Östlund, & Nilsson, 2004; Ranney et al, 2011; Törnros & Bolling, 2005; Young, 2013; see further references in ISO, 2016).…”
Section: Experimental Effects Of CL On Driving Performancementioning
confidence: 60%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Thus, according to the cognitive control hypothesis, the DRT should be sensitive to interference from cognitively loading secondary tasks. This prediction is confirmed by a large number of studies reporting that cognitively loading tasks increase DRT response times relative to a baseline (no-task) condition, with effects typically in the range of 100 to 300 ms (Bruyas & Dumont, 2013; Chong et al, 2014; Conti, Dlugosch, Vilimek, Keinath, & Bengler, 2012; Diels, 2011; Engström, Åberg, Johansson, & Hammarbäck, 2005; Engström, Larsson, & Larsson, 2013; Harbluk, Burns, Hernandez, Tam, & Glazduri, 2013; Mantzke & Keinath, 2015; Merat & Jamson, 2008; Merat, Kountouriotis, Tomlinson, Carsten, & Engström, 2015; Nilsson et al, 2017; Patten, Kircher, Östlund, & Nilsson, 2004; Ranney et al, 2011; Törnros & Bolling, 2005; Young, 2013; see further references in ISO, 2016).…”
Section: Experimental Effects Of CL On Driving Performancementioning
confidence: 60%
“…The DRT (formerly known as the Peripheral Detection Task) is an increasingly popular method specifically addressing effects of CL on OED. The method, which is defined by an international standard (ISO, 2016), involves responding to visual or tactile (and in some cases auditory; Chong, Mirchi, Silva, & Strybel, 2014) stimuli presented at intervals of 3 to 5 s. Effects of CL are measured in terms of response time or miss rate. Despite its simplicity, the DRT is typically not extensively practiced, thus not automatized, and hence relies on cognitive control (as mentioned earlier, Shiffrin and Schneider [1977] found that thousands of trials were needed for a similar task to become automatized).…”
Section: Experimental Effects Of CL On Driving Performancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…For the driving performance, the participant's position in the driving task was compared to a pre-calculated ideal path in terms of lateral deviation in meters with a 33 Hz sampling rate. A standardized procedure (ISO 26022:2010) commonly used to access the LCT driving performance [13,14,21,46,68,85]. The results were sorted into their corresponding tasks and different metrics including average deviation, median, minimum and maximum, and standard deviation.…”
Section: Data Preprocessing and Measurementsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Primary tasks that are more cognitively demanding are assumed to produce greater response latencies and more misses than tasks that are less cognitively demanding. The PDT has been used previously as a measure of workload (Chong, Mirchi, Silva, & Strybel, 2014;Van der Horst & Martens, 2010;Olsson & Burns, 2000;Patton, Kircher, Ostlund, Lilsson, & Svenson, 2006), including investigations of cognitive tunneling (e.g., Martens & Van Winsum, 2000).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%