2003
DOI: 10.1515/9783110909869
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Auch and noch in Child and Adult German

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2008
2008
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Spontaneous speech as well as experimental production data from German-and Japanese-speaking children suggest that the acquisition of both kinds of particles is achieved early. Previous research has provided evidence that from the beginning of multiword utterances German-speaking children use the additive particle auch to encode aspects of information structure (Dimroth, 2009;Höhle, Berger, Mü ller, Schmitz & Weissenborn, 2009 ;Nederstigt, 2003 ;Penner, Tracy & Weissenborn, 2000). For Japanese-speaking children the use of the additive particle mo ' also ' and the restrictive particle dake 'only ' has been observed in the productions of children from age 1 ;6-2 ;3 and from age 2 ; 1-2 ; 7 respectively, with a slight temporal delay for the restrictive particle (Matsuoka, Miyoshi, Hoshi, Ueda, Yabu & Hirata, 2006).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Spontaneous speech as well as experimental production data from German-and Japanese-speaking children suggest that the acquisition of both kinds of particles is achieved early. Previous research has provided evidence that from the beginning of multiword utterances German-speaking children use the additive particle auch to encode aspects of information structure (Dimroth, 2009;Höhle, Berger, Mü ller, Schmitz & Weissenborn, 2009 ;Nederstigt, 2003 ;Penner, Tracy & Weissenborn, 2000). For Japanese-speaking children the use of the additive particle mo ' also ' and the restrictive particle dake 'only ' has been observed in the productions of children from age 1 ;6-2 ;3 and from age 2 ; 1-2 ; 7 respectively, with a slight temporal delay for the restrictive particle (Matsuoka, Miyoshi, Hoshi, Ueda, Yabu & Hirata, 2006).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Later studies on L1- and L2-acquisition of German and Dutch confirmed these findings (L1: Nederstigt, 2003, 2006; Winkler, 2006, 2009; for a summary see Dimroth, 2009; L2: Dimroth, 2002, 2009; Dimroth, Schimke, & Verhagen, 2009; Schimke, Verhagen, & Dimroth, 2008).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 79%
“…Nederstigt (2003) finds that stressed auch in exactly this kind of information structure occurs in L1 acquisition much earlier than unstressed auch.…”
Section: The Information Structure Of Early Learner Utterances Contaimentioning
confidence: 87%
“…Such elements are not referring to anything, but rather specifying the relation between other pieces of information given in the utterance, or between information in the utterance and the (non)-verbal context. Several studies have found that for German, negative particles like nein ('no') and nicht ('not') and the additive particle auch ('also') are among the first elements of this sort (Dimroth 2002, Nederstigt 2003, Penner, Tracy, and Weissenborn 2000. It has also been suggested that utterances containing these expressions are somehow more advanced and can help the child and the untutored L2 learner to develop from the lexical structure of their early utterances towards a more target-like finite utterance structure (Dimroth et al 2003, Penner et al 2000.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation