The platform will undergo maintenance on Sep 14 at about 7:45 AM EST and will be unavailable for approximately 2 hours.
Reinforcement and Behavior 1969
DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-683650-9.50011-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Activity, Reactivity, and the Behavior-Directing Properties of Stimuli

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

3
27
0

Year Published

1972
1972
2006
2006

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 44 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
3
27
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In support of this interpretation, Shelton and Beardsley (2005) have recently shown that omission of response-contingent stimuli during testing abolishes stress-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in rats with limited access to the drug (2 h/day). Whether the permissive effect of response-contingent cues on reinstatement depends on mere sensory reinforcement (ie, Berlyne, 1969;Tapp, 1969;Gomer and Jakubczak, 1974) and/or drug conditioning is unknown at present. Nevertheless, in view of this effect, it is possible that drug-induced reinstatement is crucially dependent on response-contingent cues in ShA rats but becomes independent of these cues in LgA rats after the transition to compulsive drug use.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In support of this interpretation, Shelton and Beardsley (2005) have recently shown that omission of response-contingent stimuli during testing abolishes stress-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in rats with limited access to the drug (2 h/day). Whether the permissive effect of response-contingent cues on reinstatement depends on mere sensory reinforcement (ie, Berlyne, 1969;Tapp, 1969;Gomer and Jakubczak, 1974) and/or drug conditioning is unknown at present. Nevertheless, in view of this effect, it is possible that drug-induced reinstatement is crucially dependent on response-contingent cues in ShA rats but becomes independent of these cues in LgA rats after the transition to compulsive drug use.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…in the absence of response-contingent stimuli. This within-session reinstatement procedure allowed one to measure the priming or reinstating effects of the drug, without the potential confounding influence of unconditioned and/or conditioned sensory reinforcement (eg, Berlyne, 1969;Tapp, 1969;Gomer and Jakubczak, 1974;Robbins and Koob, 1978). The sensitization hypothesis predicts that LgA rats should be more responsive than ShA rats to both the psychomotor and priming effects of cocaine.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…, faster run time closer to the goal, may be an "artifact" of competing behavior (King, 1959;Marx & Brownstein, 1963 (Bruce, 1937;Crespi, 1942;Hull, 1934), and that a sharp goal gradient returns 13 early in extinction (Hull, 1934;Miller & Miles, 1935 on behavior (Brown, 1961;Campbell & Cicala, 1962;Petrinovich & Bolles, 1954;Tapp, 1969), which is implicit in Hull's (1943) "big D," generalized drive; (b) that all reinforcers operate in the same way in all learning situations (Seligman, 1970;Shettleworth, 1972;Teitelbaum, 1966). These assumptions have been borne out in many situations for thirsty animals performing for water (e. g. , Jenkins & Arnold, 1968;Kintsch, 1962; McCoy Marx, 1965;Skinner, 1938;Weinstock, 1958;Zimmerman, 1971) (Logan & Spanier, 1970).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Also, in an alleyway study using thirsty rats, Kintsch (1962) Kintsch (1962). that competing behavior does occur throughout training for thirsty rats (Bindra, 1963;McCoy & Marx, 1965 Petrinovich & Bolles, 1954;Shettleworth, 1972;Tapp, 1969 …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation