1976
DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.105.3.277
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A fragmentation hypothesis of memory: Cued recall of pictures and of sequential position.

Abstract: It is a common assumption that memories consist of sets of interrelated features or attributes. Rigorous and direct investigation of the functional properties of these sets has been relatively neglected however. Two important questions are posed here. First, are the interrelationships of different components reflexive or are they asymmetric? Second, how do components interact when more than one is used as a cue for recall ?These questions were answered by examining memory for series of items whose objective co… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

12
146
1

Year Published

1979
1979
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 155 publications
(159 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
12
146
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The name of someone one met on a single occasion is a good example of both asymmetries (a good cue, but sometimes hard to retrieve), while the location of the meeting is often both a good cue and relatively easy to retrieve. The sequential position of an item in a list of items is another example of a good cue that is relatively hard to retrieve itself (Jones 1976). Considerations such as these demand at least asymmetrical associations, and have fuelled the theoretical distinctions made between the context and content of an event (e.g.…”
Section: A Generic Hippocampo-neocortical Model Of Long-term Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The name of someone one met on a single occasion is a good example of both asymmetries (a good cue, but sometimes hard to retrieve), while the location of the meeting is often both a good cue and relatively easy to retrieve. The sequential position of an item in a list of items is another example of a good cue that is relatively hard to retrieve itself (Jones 1976). Considerations such as these demand at least asymmetrical associations, and have fuelled the theoretical distinctions made between the context and content of an event (e.g.…”
Section: A Generic Hippocampo-neocortical Model Of Long-term Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…That is, for some types of material, the fragment model may, in general, represent recall better than does the schema model. Previous work (Jones, 1976(Jones, , 1978c had suggested that the fragment model is successful in representing the recall of material with unrelated components. Consequently, in the experiments to be reported here, participants attempted to recall clusters of words that had not been selected on the basis of their preexisting relations to each other.…”
Section: Comparison Of the Modelsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, they did not note that, in fact, the first and second are different members of the same family of theories, treated here as the horizontal and general versions, respectively, of the fragment model. The general version is the form in which the fragment model has usually been tested previously (e.g., Jones, 1976Jones, , 1978cJones & Payne, 1982). The four different versions are described next.…”
Section: Fragment Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations