2011
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00275
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A Cognitive Architecture for the Coordination of Utterances

Abstract: Dialog partners coordinate with each other to reach a common goal. The analogy with other joint activities has sparked interesting observations (e.g., about the norms governing turn-taking) and has informed studies of linguistic alignment in dialog. However, the parallels between language and action have not been fully explored, especially with regard to the mechanisms that support moment-by-moment coordination during language use in conversation. We review the literature on joint actions to show (i) what sort… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
28
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(31 citation statements)
references
References 96 publications
3
28
0
Order By: Relevance
“…First, researchers in these areas are beginning to tap into the cognitive mechanisms that might underlie social and linguistic interaction (e.g. Brown-Schmidt, 2009a,b;Gambi & Pickering, 2011;Horton, 2005;Mehler, Weiß, Menke, & Lücking, 2010;Reitter, Keller, & Moore, 2011). This advances the valuable work on observational and conversation analysis that has shed great light on the structure of interaction (Sacks, Jefferson, & Schegloff, 1995;Schegloff, 2007), but is not capable of identifying the cognitive processes that drive it.…”
Section: -Plm59c-9780124071872mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, researchers in these areas are beginning to tap into the cognitive mechanisms that might underlie social and linguistic interaction (e.g. Brown-Schmidt, 2009a,b;Gambi & Pickering, 2011;Horton, 2005;Mehler, Weiß, Menke, & Lücking, 2010;Reitter, Keller, & Moore, 2011). This advances the valuable work on observational and conversation analysis that has shed great light on the structure of interaction (Sacks, Jefferson, & Schegloff, 1995;Schegloff, 2007), but is not capable of identifying the cognitive processes that drive it.…”
Section: -Plm59c-9780124071872mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…According to Pickering and Garrod (2007) (see also, Gambi & Pickering, 2011;Pickering & Garrod, 2013, for reviews) the language production system generates forward models (imitative plans) at specific levels of representation, including semantics, syntax, and phonology, to predict utterances during language comprehension. However, apart from studies revealing the involvement of motor processes during speech perception (e.g., Fadiga, Craighero, Buccino, & Rizzolatti, 2002; motor theories of speech perception, see Hickok, 2012), little is known about the exact role that our production system plays in predicting others' utterances, especially at more abstract levels of representation (e.g., lexical) where articulation is not present.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, rather than making such a strong claim about the absolute time course of the unfolding form-prediction, we think that slower SOAs might in principle benefit all aspects of prediction. After all, people are more likely to finish the sentences of someone who speaks slowly or hesitantly than of someone who speaks fast and fluently (see Gambi & Pickering, 2011).…”
Section: Conditionmentioning
confidence: 99%