1984
DOI: 10.1080/03610738408258544
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Younger and older adults' attention to relevant and irrelevant stimuli in free recall

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

1985
1985
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 6 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We concluded our search in February 2009. Studies were included (a) if they examined at least one sample of younger (mean age <30 years) and one sample of older (mean age >60 years) adults, (b) if the data were reported in a format amenable to meta-analysis (e.g., mean and standard deviation or standard error, F ratio, t test, standardized difference), (c) if the study was published in the English language in a peer-reviewed journal, (d) if the dependent measure pertained to episodic memory accuracy (this excluded studies that applied short-term-memory paradigms, typically yielding latency measures; Oberauer, 2001, 2005a, 2005b; Witthöft et al, 2009; Zacks et al, 1996, Experiment 3), and (e) if the scoring allowed for a comparison between TBR and TBF materials (this excluded Andrés, Van der Linden, & Parmentier, 2004; one experimental condition from Collette, Germain, et al, 2009; Collette, Schmidt, Scherrer, Adam, & Salmon, 2009; Pavur et al, 1984). The final sample consisted of 10 articles, containing 24 different experiments or independent participant groups; the studies are reported in Table 1.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…We concluded our search in February 2009. Studies were included (a) if they examined at least one sample of younger (mean age <30 years) and one sample of older (mean age >60 years) adults, (b) if the data were reported in a format amenable to meta-analysis (e.g., mean and standard deviation or standard error, F ratio, t test, standardized difference), (c) if the study was published in the English language in a peer-reviewed journal, (d) if the dependent measure pertained to episodic memory accuracy (this excluded studies that applied short-term-memory paradigms, typically yielding latency measures; Oberauer, 2001, 2005a, 2005b; Witthöft et al, 2009; Zacks et al, 1996, Experiment 3), and (e) if the scoring allowed for a comparison between TBR and TBF materials (this excluded Andrés, Van der Linden, & Parmentier, 2004; one experimental condition from Collette, Germain, et al, 2009; Collette, Schmidt, Scherrer, Adam, & Salmon, 2009; Pavur et al, 1984). The final sample consisted of 10 articles, containing 24 different experiments or independent participant groups; the studies are reported in Table 1.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…2A, 2B; Zellner & Bäuml, 2006, Exp. 1-3); there are, however, exceptions to this pattern (Gamboz & Russo, 2002; Pavur, Comeaux, & Zeringue, 1984; Sahakyan et al, 2008a, Exp. 1, Exp 2: context change instructions; Zacks et al, 1996, Exp.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Because such situations are likely to be common in the life experience of older as well as younger adults, it is perhaps surprising that the literature contains few studies of directed forgetting in older adults. The ones that we are aware of include a study that used the unusual procedure of cueing participants before study about the TBR-TBF status of individual items (Pavur, Comeaux, & Zeringue, 1984), a second that was reported at a conference and is briefly summarized in a book chapter (Camp & McKitrick, 1989), and a third that was the basis of another conference report (Giambra & Howard, 1994). Given this situation (few studies and incomplete reports of all but one), it seems premature to reach even tentative conclusions about directed forgetting effects in older adults without additional research.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%