2009
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02246.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

You Can't Always Get What You Want

Abstract: When do infants understand that goals exist independently of the actions that result from them? Exploring infants' understanding of failed intentional actions-when the goal of the action is unfulfilled and thus non-apparent in the actor's movements-critically addresses this question. Using a visual habituation paradigm, we assessed when infants understand that a failed intentional action is goal-directed and whether an understanding of successful intentional actions (actions that do overtly attain their goals)… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
51
0
2

Year Published

2010
2010
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 163 publications
(62 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
5
51
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…However, in real life, features such as the goal-directedness of the action performed, its controllability (Weiner, 1995), or the agents’s emotional states (e.g., whether she/he is surprised or happy about the results of her/his action; Behne, Carpenter and Tomasello, 2005) may be critical in determining saliency and helping individuals to determine whether someone acts intentionally or not. Since most of the critical features that feed into these decisions are processed using lower-level mechanism that are available early during the development (Brandone & Wellman, 2009; Cannon & Woodward 2012), it is possible that information about agents’ intentions are incorporated much more easily in daily-life settings than in the experimental settings.…”
Section: Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, in real life, features such as the goal-directedness of the action performed, its controllability (Weiner, 1995), or the agents’s emotional states (e.g., whether she/he is surprised or happy about the results of her/his action; Behne, Carpenter and Tomasello, 2005) may be critical in determining saliency and helping individuals to determine whether someone acts intentionally or not. Since most of the critical features that feed into these decisions are processed using lower-level mechanism that are available early during the development (Brandone & Wellman, 2009; Cannon & Woodward 2012), it is possible that information about agents’ intentions are incorporated much more easily in daily-life settings than in the experimental settings.…”
Section: Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In visual habituation experiments, when infants are habituated to repeated examples of an agent acting on a particular goal, for example grasping a ball, they subsequently show longer looking (a response to novelty) to events that disrupt the original relationship between the agent and her goal as compared to events that preserve this relationship while varying the physical details of the agent's movements (see [20] for a review). This pattern of response is evident by three to six months of age for simple instrumental actions, like grasping [21][22][23][24], and by nine to 12 months of age for actions that relate agents to objects at a distance or indirectly, like looking and pointing [25][26][27][28][29], reaching over obstacles [30] or using a tool or means to attain an object [31 -35].…”
Section: Infants' Analysis Of Others' Actionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, infants begin pointing to objects communicatively (Butterworth, 2003;Carpenter et al, 1998); they become upset when their goals are blocked and are pleased when they achieve an intended goal (Fogel, 2011); they seek appraisal from caregivers to regulate their own emotions (Mumme & Fernald, 2003;Sorce, Emde, Campos, & Klinnert, 1985); and they expect social partners to express interest in shared referents . Converging evidence is provided by imitation studies, where infants by 10-12 months of age observe the unsuccessful attempts of a model to perform a goal-directed action (e.g., pulling apart a toy barbell) and then perform the target act in spite of not seeing it (Brandone & Wellman, 2009;Legerstee & Markova, 2008;Meltzoff, 1995). These results suggest that infants can infer the unseen goals or intentions of the model; otherwise, it would not have been possible for them to perform the intended actions.…”
Section: Dyadic and Triadic Interactionsmentioning
confidence: 88%