2012
DOI: 10.1037/a0029872
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Word learning emerges from the interaction of online referent selection and slow associative learning.

Abstract: Classic approaches to word learning emphasize the problem of referential ambiguity: in any naming situation the referent of a novel word must be selected from many possible objects, properties, actions, etc. To solve this problem, researchers have posited numerous constraints, and inference strategies, but assume that determining the referent of a novel word is isomorphic to learning. We present an alternative model in which referent selection is an online process that is independent of long-term learning. Thi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

30
428
3
3

Year Published

2013
2013
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

3
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 330 publications
(464 citation statements)
references
References 178 publications
(451 reference statements)
30
428
3
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Nonetheless, not all infants responded correctly to the label, and some shifted their attention away from the target. We do not therefore claim that these prespeech infants learned a new word during training, but rather we interpret these results as supporting accounts of early word learning in which infants learn label‐object associations incrementally (Bion, Borovsky, & Fernald, 2013; McMurray, Horst, & Samuelson, 2012; Yurovsky, Fricker, Yu, & Smith, 2014). That is, these infants learned something about the object, something about the label, and something about the mapping between the two.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 55%
“…Nonetheless, not all infants responded correctly to the label, and some shifted their attention away from the target. We do not therefore claim that these prespeech infants learned a new word during training, but rather we interpret these results as supporting accounts of early word learning in which infants learn label‐object associations incrementally (Bion, Borovsky, & Fernald, 2013; McMurray, Horst, & Samuelson, 2012; Yurovsky, Fricker, Yu, & Smith, 2014). That is, these infants learned something about the object, something about the label, and something about the mapping between the two.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 55%
“…N recognise the novel words was typically better than their ability to recall and produce the novel words (Brown et al, 2012;Henderson et al, 2012Henderson et al, , 2013aDumay & Gaskell, 2007;McMurray et al, 2012); however, children only recalled 11% of novel words and adults recalled only 29% of novel words immediately after training. Children and adults of a similar age range in Henderson et al (2013a) showed mean cued recall rates of 18% and 61%…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because the word layer used a winner-take-all mechanism, a small difference in activation could lead one unit to be selected over another. Thus, this part of the architecture instantiated the competition between referents and learned concept-word associations specified by both theories of word learning (McMurray, Horst, & Samuelson, 2012) and word production (Gordon & Dell, 2003).…”
Section: Model Architecturementioning
confidence: 99%