2017
DOI: 10.1111/infa.12201
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learned Labels Shape Pre‐speech Infants’ Object Representations

Abstract: Infants rapidly learn both linguistic and nonlinguistic representations of their environment and begin to link these from around 6 months. While there is an increasing body of evidence for the effect of labels heard in‐task on infants’ online processing, whether infants’ learned linguistic representations shape learned nonlinguistic representations is unclear. In this study 10‐month‐old infants were trained over the course of a week with two 3D objects, one labeled, and one unlabeled. Infants then took part in… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

1
25
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
1
25
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, variability supports category learning in infants and adults (Goldenberg & Sandhofer, ; Smith & Handy, ). Infants show better retention of object labels if they experience object–label mappings against a changing background across trials, rather than one that remains constant across trials (Twomey & Westermann, ). In learning paradigms with adults, variation in context is associated with better learning of grammatical class (Redington, Chater, & Finch, ) and referent mappings (Smith & Yu, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, variability supports category learning in infants and adults (Goldenberg & Sandhofer, ; Smith & Handy, ). Infants show better retention of object labels if they experience object–label mappings against a changing background across trials, rather than one that remains constant across trials (Twomey & Westermann, ). In learning paradigms with adults, variation in context is associated with better learning of grammatical class (Redington, Chater, & Finch, ) and referent mappings (Smith & Yu, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Effects of learning history also emerge when infants' experience is controlled experimentally. For example, after a week of training with one named and one unnamed novel object, 10-month-old infants exhibited increased visual sampling of the previously named object in a subsequent silent lookingtime task (Twomey & Westermann, 2017; see also Bornstein & Mash, 2010;Gliga, Volein, & Csibra, 2010). Thus, learning depends on the interaction between what infants encounter in-the-moment and what they know (Thelen & Smith, 1994).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is possible, however, that infants' prior familiarity with either the object or the label may have influenced the ease with which they learned the two word-object associations in the current study (Fennell, 2011;Kucker & Samuelson, 2012). Indeed, existing studies with novel objects and labels show that 10-month-old infants can learn novel word-object associations after substantially more training (Twomey & Westermann, 2018), or demonstrate rapid word-learning in older infants of 14 and 15 months of age (Schafer & Plunkett, 1998;Werker et al, 1998). Whether infants are similarly able to rapidly learn two completely novel word-object associations when the objects and the labels are completely unfamiliar remains an interesting challenge for future work.…”
Section: Early Success In Word Learning and Generalizationmentioning
confidence: 79%
“…Bergelson and Swingley (2012) and Syrnyk and Meints (2017) have demonstrated word recognition for several common objects in infants at or before 9 months of age, documenting the formation of word-object associations through daily experience with language. Further, Twomey and Westermann (2018) demonstrated that 10-month-old infants could learn novel-label to novel-object mappings when trained with objects and labels over the course of a week.…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%