2019
DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2019.1670184
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Limits of Infants’ Early Word Learning

Abstract: In this series of experiments, we tested the limits of young infants' word learning and generalization abilities in light of recent findings reporting sophisticated word learning abilities in the first year of life. Ten-month-old infants were trained with two word-object pairs and tested with either the same or different members of the corresponding categories. In Experiment 1, infants showed successful learning of the word-object associations, when trained and tested with a single exemplar from each category.… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
17
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
(51 reference statements)
1
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Our daily lives are full of sensory input from different modalities and language provides us with the means to describe those rich experiences, with words for visual, auditory, olfactory and haptic experiences such as flower (visual), thunder (auditory), stench (olfactory), and tickle (haptic). Despite this rich and multimodal environment, research on how we acquire words for entities and events has mostly focused on labeling objects in the visual modality (e.g., Borgström et al, 2015a ; Friedrich and Friederici, 2008 ; Horst and Samuelson, 2008 ; Junge et al, 2012 ; Pruden et al, 2006 ; Smith and Yu, 2008 ; Taxitari et al, 2019 ; Torkildsen et al, 2008 , 2007 ; Werker et al, 1998 ). This imbalance may originate from the larger number of words for visible things in many languages.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our daily lives are full of sensory input from different modalities and language provides us with the means to describe those rich experiences, with words for visual, auditory, olfactory and haptic experiences such as flower (visual), thunder (auditory), stench (olfactory), and tickle (haptic). Despite this rich and multimodal environment, research on how we acquire words for entities and events has mostly focused on labeling objects in the visual modality (e.g., Borgström et al, 2015a ; Friedrich and Friederici, 2008 ; Horst and Samuelson, 2008 ; Junge et al, 2012 ; Pruden et al, 2006 ; Smith and Yu, 2008 ; Taxitari et al, 2019 ; Torkildsen et al, 2008 , 2007 ; Werker et al, 1998 ). This imbalance may originate from the larger number of words for visible things in many languages.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This protocol was used to reduce the infants’ cognitive demands because the learning of new word-visual referent associations learning at the laboratory has been described as hard even in non-fluent contexts 40 . Previous studies in young infants reported that more complex tasks such as word-object categorization are accompanied by lower accuracy and gaze’s long latency starting 1.5 s after the onset of the object presentation 40 . The long latency to switch the gaze toward the selected image we observed in all our experiments supports this idea.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These findings stand in slight contrast to the literature on word learning. Research suggests that when presented with only a word for an object, children as young as 9 to 10 months of age learn to associate words with objects ( [38], but see [39] that such learning is contingent on a number of factors, e.g., similarity of the two objects presented). These studies, however, only presented infants with word-object associations and not both word-and action-object associations.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%