2020
DOI: 10.3390/brainsci10080517
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Whole-Language and Item-Specific Inhibition in Bilingual Language Switching: The Role of Domain–General Inhibitory Control

Abstract: A prominent theory of bilingual speech production holds that appropriate language selection is achieved via inhibitory control. Such inhibition may operate on the whole-language and/or item-specific level. In this study, we examined these two levels of control in parallel, by introducing a novel element into the traditional cued language switching paradigm: half of the stimuli were univalent (each required naming in the same language every time it appeared), and the other half were bivalent (each required nami… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 80 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Hence, by having a relatively higher selection threshold for L1 than L2, a reversed language dominance pattern should be observed. Though, since Costa and Santesteban (2004) assumed that only highly proficient bilinguals are able to change the selection thresholds for each language, a reversed language dominance pattern should only be observed with highly proficient bilinguals, which does not seem to be the case (e.g., Costa & Santesteban, 2004, Experiment 1; Liu et al, 2016; Zhu & Sowman, 2020). Therefore, the assumption that language proficiency is a major factor in the occurrence/absence of the reversed language dominance effect, as suggested by Costa and Santesteban (2004), seems unlikely.…”
Section: Inhibitory Phenomena In Bilingual Language Productionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Hence, by having a relatively higher selection threshold for L1 than L2, a reversed language dominance pattern should be observed. Though, since Costa and Santesteban (2004) assumed that only highly proficient bilinguals are able to change the selection thresholds for each language, a reversed language dominance pattern should only be observed with highly proficient bilinguals, which does not seem to be the case (e.g., Costa & Santesteban, 2004, Experiment 1; Liu et al, 2016; Zhu & Sowman, 2020). Therefore, the assumption that language proficiency is a major factor in the occurrence/absence of the reversed language dominance effect, as suggested by Costa and Santesteban (2004), seems unlikely.…”
Section: Inhibitory Phenomena In Bilingual Language Productionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As we move toward harnessing the insights for application/intervention from the basic science research looking into potential connections between bilingualism and cognitive aging, research that employs other methods, such as Trancranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) and/or Transcranial magnetic Stimulation (TMS), are also welcome. While these methods have both basic science application and therapeutic potential, especially as paired with cognitive training for the functional connectivity of working memory in the elderly (Nissim et al, 2019;Indahlastari et al, 2021), and although they have been used in bilingual language control research showing some promising effects in studies focusing on younger bilinguals (Hämäläinen et al, 2018;Radman et al, 2018;Jost et al, 2020;Tong et al, 2020;Zhu and Sowman, 2020;Vaughn et al, 2021), these methods are virtually unknown in the literature on bilingualism and cognitive aging.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We predict that auditory cues like 'say' used by Hernandez and colleagues (1999Hernandez and colleagues ( , 2000Hernandez and colleagues ( , 2001 similarly activate the speech goal as question cues used in the present study and by (Tarlowski et al, 2013). To understand whether speech production in response to an auditory cues is crucial to capture BLC, the auditory questions could be compared to visual questions as used by Zhu and Sowman (2020). Furthermore, to understand whether a communicative cue, like a question or the word 'say', has an additional benefit over non-communicative words could be tested by comparing…”
mentioning
confidence: 82%