2023
DOI: 10.1037/rev0000367
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The concept of inhibition in bilingual control.

Abstract: To achieve fluent language processing as a bilingual, a dominant theoretical framework assumes that the nontarget language is inhibited. This assumption is based on several empirical effects that are typically explained with inhibitory control. In the current article, we discuss four prominent effects linked to bilingual inhibition in language production (i.e., asymmetrical switch costs, n−2 language repetition costs, reversed language dominance, and the blocked language order effect). We argue that these effe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

5
31
2

Year Published

2023
2023
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 30 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 203 publications
(456 reference statements)
5
31
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Explanations that assume the involvement of proactive control have been previously used to account for the L2 after-effect Declerck & Koch, 2022), and our current findings are generally consistent with this framework. However, they also point to an important caveat regarding the level of the targeted representations.…”
Section: Putative Explanations Of the L2 After-effectsupporting
confidence: 88%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Explanations that assume the involvement of proactive control have been previously used to account for the L2 after-effect Declerck & Koch, 2022), and our current findings are generally consistent with this framework. However, they also point to an important caveat regarding the level of the targeted representations.…”
Section: Putative Explanations Of the L2 After-effectsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…While these accounts provide plausible explanations of this effect, no univocal evidence in favor or against these interpretations has been provided (Casado et al, 2022; Wodniecka, Szewczyk, et al, 2020; Declerck & Koch, 2022). All available accounts of the L2 after-effect build on a common theoretical assumption that the difficulty in producing words in L1 after using L2 is a consequence of a control mechanism that changes the balance between the relative activation of L1 and L2 representations (Casado et al, 2022; for a discussion see Declerck & Koch, 2022). Importantly, according to these accounts the change in L1-L2 balance results in increased interference between the two languages that persists for some time after the speaker switches back to speaking in L1 (Casado et al, 2022), which, in turn, increases the difficulty of access and selection of (a) lexico-semantic representations (e.g., Casado et al, 2022) and/or (b) phonological representations and articulatory programs of L1 (e.g., Guo et al, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Along the same lines, it is still unclear what the boundary conditions are to observe asymmetrical switch costs (for reviews, see [ 4 , 27 , 58 ]). So, finding symmetrical switch costs might also be due to characteristics of this study other than voluntary language switching.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Consistent with previous findings (e.g., Costa & Santesteban, 2004;Declerck et al, 2015;Gollan & Ferreira, 2009;Li & Gollan, 2018), this result suggests that unbalanced bilinguals exerted strong inhibition on the dominant language during the mixed language naming condition. It should be noted that there are different interpretations of the reversed language dominance effect: While some researchers argue it is a strong indicator of inhibition over the dominant language (e.g., Bobb & Wodniecka, 2013;Fu et al, 2017), other scholars suggest it could also be explained with alternative mechanisms (see Gade et al, 2021aGade et al, , 2021b for related discussions), such as persistent over-activation (e.g., Declerck et al, 2015;Declerck & Koch, 2023) or lower selection threshold for the weaker L2 (Costa & Santesteban, 2004). We suppose this effect reflects inhibition over the L1, as it converges with our ERP decoding data discussed below.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%