2013
DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2013.765388
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Who am I?: The influence of affect on the working self-concept

Abstract: Two experiments investigated the impact of affect on the working self-concept. Following an affect induction, participants completed the twenty statements test (TST) to assess their working self-concepts. Participants in predominantly happy and angry states used more abstract statements to describe themselves than did participants in predominantly sad and fearful states. Evaluations of the statements that participants generated (Experiment 2) demonstrate that these effects are not the result of (1) participant… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
30
0
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(32 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
1
30
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Relatedly, Beukeboom and Semin (2006) found that the influence of affect-induced global and local processing styles led happy participants to describe a film in more abstract language than sad participants; however, this difference was eliminated when the source of participants’ affect was made salient. Isbell et al (2011) reported a similar effect in an investigation of the influence of affective states on individuals’ spontaneous self-descriptions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 76%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Relatedly, Beukeboom and Semin (2006) found that the influence of affect-induced global and local processing styles led happy participants to describe a film in more abstract language than sad participants; however, this difference was eliminated when the source of participants’ affect was made salient. Isbell et al (2011) reported a similar effect in an investigation of the influence of affective states on individuals’ spontaneous self-descriptions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 76%
“…Evidence of this tendency is ubiquitous. Compared to individuals in sad (or neutral) moods, those in happy moods rely on stereotypes to a greater extent during impression formation (Bless, 2000; Bodenhausen, Kramer, & Susser, 1994; Isbell, 2004), actively select global traits over behaviors when forming impressions of others (Isbell, Burns, & Haar, 2005), rely on scripts (Bless et al, 1996), are less influenced by argument strength in persuasion tasks (e.g., Bless, Bohner, Schwarz, & Strack, 1990; Schwarz, Bless, & Bohner, 1991), create and use categories more flexibly (Isen & Daubman, 1984; Isen, Daubman, & Nowicki, 1987; Murray, Sujan, Hirt, & Sujan, 1990), and describe behaviors, events, and themselves in more abstract language (Beukeboom & Semin, 2005,2006; Isbell, McCabe, Burns, & Lair, 2011). Happy moods also increase the likelihood of the fundamental attribution error (Forgas, 1998), an error that results when individuals fail to correct their global dispositional judgments for situational details.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Predictions derived from the motivational intensity model also appear inconsistent with at least some past research. For example, the motivational intensity model predicts that anger, an experience of high motiva tional intensity, narrows attention, yet research discussed earlier (e.g., Isbell, McCabe, et al, 2013) demonstrates instead that anger widens conceptual attention. Sad ness, an experience of low motivational intensity, is frequently found to narrow attention (Biss & Hasher, 2011;Gasper, 2004;Gasper & Clore, 2002;Kuhbandner et al, 2011;Rowe et al, 2007), yet the motivational intensity view predicts increased per ceptual and cognitive broadening.…”
Section: Motivational Intensity Modelmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Change in the emotional content of the self-concept was assessed before training onset and at the end of each training module by means of emotional word use extracted from the Twenty Statement Test (TST) (Kuhn & McPartland, 1954). The open-ended response format of the TST allows to measure different structural features of the self-concept, including the emotional content of the self-concept (Isbell, McCabe, Burns, & Lair, 2013;Krans et al, 2015). The emotional content of the selfconcept was operationalized with emotional words used in the self-descriptions of the TST.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%