2014
DOI: 10.1017/s1355617714000228
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Which Language Declines More? Longitudinal versus Cross-sectional Decline of Picture Naming in Bilinguals with Alzheimer’s Disease

Abstract: In this study, we investigated dual-language decline in non-balanced bilinguals with probable Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) both longitudinally and cross-sectionally. We examined patients’ naming accuracy on the Boston Naming Test (BNT: Kaplan et al., 1983) over three testing sessions (longitudinal analysis) and compared their performance to that of matched controls (cross-sectional analysis). We found different longitudinal and cross-sectional patterns of decline: Longitudinally, the non-dominant language seemed t… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
27
2

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
(71 reference statements)
0
27
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Previously we proposed that the dominant language is more sensitive to subtle differences between patients and controls that emerge in early stages of AD possibly because AD impairs semantic processing (reviewed above; see also Butters et al, 1987; Monsch et al, 1994; Salmon, Butters, & Chan, 1999) and the dominant language has richer semantic representations (Gollan et al, 2010), or because AD produces a general retrieval deficit and the most difficult-to-retrieve words that bilinguals know belong to the dominant language (Ivanova et al, 2014). The semantic account is challenged by finding that bilinguals with AD produced more intrusion errors than controls.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Previously we proposed that the dominant language is more sensitive to subtle differences between patients and controls that emerge in early stages of AD possibly because AD impairs semantic processing (reviewed above; see also Butters et al, 1987; Monsch et al, 1994; Salmon, Butters, & Chan, 1999) and the dominant language has richer semantic representations (Gollan et al, 2010), or because AD produces a general retrieval deficit and the most difficult-to-retrieve words that bilinguals know belong to the dominant language (Ivanova et al, 2014). The semantic account is challenged by finding that bilinguals with AD produced more intrusion errors than controls.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This pattern was apparent in the initial stages of disease progression in bilinguals who had one clearly more proficient language (Ivanova, Salmon, & Gollan, 2014; Kowoll, Degen, Gladdis, & Schröder, 2015), whereas balanced bilinguals exhibited parallel decline of both languages (Costa et al, 2012; Salvatierra, Rosselli, Acevedo, & Duara, 2007). This finding was counterintuitive because naming deficits are usually more pronounced for low-frequency than high-frequency words (Hodges, et al, 1992; Kirshner, Webb, & Kelly, 1984; Ober & Shenaut, 1988; Thomspon-Schill, Gabrieli, & Fleischman, 1999) and bilinguals generally speak their non-dominant language less frequently than their dominant language.…”
mentioning
confidence: 94%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The authors suggest that these seemingly contradictory results show that both languages are affected by AD, but decline in a different way depending on the stage of the disease's progression, with the dominant language being affected first, because it contains the most complex and weakly represented words (Ivanova, Salmon, & Gollan, 2014).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Based on caregivers' reports, they concluded that the non-dominant language was more affected than the dominant language. Ivanova et al (2014) found different longitudinal and cross-sectional patterns of decline. The non-dominant language declined more than the dominant language, but differences between patients and controls were greater for the dominant than for the non-dominant language.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 93%