2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.05.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

When bilinguals choose a single word to speak: Electrophysiological evidence for inhibition of the native language

Abstract: Behavioral and event-related potential (ERP) measures are reported for a study in which relatively proficient Chinese-English bilinguals named identical pictures in each of their two languages. Production occurred only in Chinese (the first language, L1) or only in English (the second language, L2) in a given block with the order counterbalanced across participants. The repetition of pictures across blocks was expected to produce facilitation in the form of faster responses and more positive ERPs. However, we … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3

Citation Types

27
240
6
14

Year Published

2012
2012
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 239 publications
(287 citation statements)
references
References 77 publications
27
240
6
14
Order By: Relevance
“…However, it is still debated whether lexical selection, too, is non-specific with respect to language. Some studies suggest that lexical entries / representations from both languages compete for selection, and that to resolve this competition, the non-target language is actively inhibited (Abutalebi & Green, 2007;Costa, Colomé, Gómez, & Sebastián-Gallés, 2003;Green, 1998;Hermans et al, 1998;Jacobs, Fricke, & Kroll, 2016;Kroll, Bobb, Misra, & Guo, 2008;Misra, Guo, Bobb, & Kroll, 2012;Spalek, Hoshino, Wu, Damian, & Thierry, 2014), while others advocate language-specific lexical selection in which no competition for selection arises between two languages (Colomé, 2001;Costa, Miozzo, & Caramazza, 1999). More recently, it has been proposed that lexical selection in bilingual language production may be thought of as a dynamic process, in which language-selectivity can be achieved temporarily depending on a number of variables both specific to the speakers involved (e.g.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, it is still debated whether lexical selection, too, is non-specific with respect to language. Some studies suggest that lexical entries / representations from both languages compete for selection, and that to resolve this competition, the non-target language is actively inhibited (Abutalebi & Green, 2007;Costa, Colomé, Gómez, & Sebastián-Gallés, 2003;Green, 1998;Hermans et al, 1998;Jacobs, Fricke, & Kroll, 2016;Kroll, Bobb, Misra, & Guo, 2008;Misra, Guo, Bobb, & Kroll, 2012;Spalek, Hoshino, Wu, Damian, & Thierry, 2014), while others advocate language-specific lexical selection in which no competition for selection arises between two languages (Colomé, 2001;Costa, Miozzo, & Caramazza, 1999). More recently, it has been proposed that lexical selection in bilingual language production may be thought of as a dynamic process, in which language-selectivity can be achieved temporarily depending on a number of variables both specific to the speakers involved (e.g.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, bilingual speakers have been shown to use inhibition to suppress a non-target language (e.g., De Bruin et al, 2014;Guo et al, 2011;Misra et al, 2012;Jackson et al, 2001;Roelofs et al, 2011;Verhoef et al, 2009). There is also evidence suggesting that inhibition deficits contribute to the impaired speech production of children with specific language impairment (SLI; e.g., Henry et al, 2012;Im-Bolter et al, 2006;Seiger-Gardner and Schwartz, 2008;Spaulding, 2010).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Liu, Misra, & Kroll, 2011;Jackson, Swainson, Cunnington, & Jackson, 2001;Misra, Guo, Bobb, & Kroll, 2012;Roelofs, Piai, & Garrido Rodriguez, 2011). Moreover, there is evidence that inhibition deficits contribute to the impaired word production of children with developmental language disorders, such as specific language impairment (e.g., Henry, Messer, & Nash, 2012;Im-Bolter, Johnson, & Pascual-Leone, 2006;SeigerGardner & Schwartz, 2008;Spaulding, 2010).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Much of the work on the role of inhibition in word production has concerned bilingual speakers. A common assumption is that bilingual speakers use inhibition to suppress words in the nontarget language, either obligatorily (Abutalebi & Green, 2007;Costa, Hernández, & Sebastián-Gallés, 2008;Green, 1998;Guo et al, 2011;Jackson et al, 2001;Misra et al, 2012) or optionally (Roelofs et al, 2011;Verhoef, Roelofs, & Chwilla, 2009). Because of the routine engagement of inhibition in language control, bilingual speakers might outperform monolingual speakers in linguistic as well as nonlinguistic tasks involving inhibitory control.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%