2012
DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2011.644798
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What you Say Matters: Exploring Visual–Verbal Interactions in Visual Working Memory

Abstract: The aim of this study was to explore whether the content of a simple concurrent verbal load task determines the extent of its interference on memory for coloured shapes. The task consisted of remembering four visual items while repeating aloud a pair of words that varied in terms of imageability and relatedness to the task set. At test, a cue appeared that was either the colour or the shape of one of the previously seen objects, with participants required to select the object's other feature from a visual arra… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

1
23
0
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
1
23
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Moreover, in each of these studies demonstrating effects of verbalization, stimuli that were amenable to verbalization (determined by pilot testing) were chosen. In an investigation of effects of articulation on color-shape memory in which only articulation of visually imaginable phrases harmed visual recognition, participants were given 4 seconds, one second per visual object, to study the objects for a later memory test (Mate et al 2012). In contrast, the stimulus presentation timings we employed (100–300 ms per item, depending on whether inter-stimulus intervals are considered) were substantially faster than those used in paradigms meant to encourage verbalization, and our stimuli were random patterns of colors.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Moreover, in each of these studies demonstrating effects of verbalization, stimuli that were amenable to verbalization (determined by pilot testing) were chosen. In an investigation of effects of articulation on color-shape memory in which only articulation of visually imaginable phrases harmed visual recognition, participants were given 4 seconds, one second per visual object, to study the objects for a later memory test (Mate et al 2012). In contrast, the stimulus presentation timings we employed (100–300 ms per item, depending on whether inter-stimulus intervals are considered) were substantially faster than those used in paradigms meant to encourage verbalization, and our stimuli were random patterns of colors.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The use of precautionary articulatory suppression is common practice despite evidence that articulatory suppression has not been shown to have a measurable effect on some visual change detection tasks (Luria, Sessa, Gotler, Jolicoeur, & Dell’Acqua, 2010; Mate, Allen, & Baqués, 2012; Morey & Cowan, 2004, 2005), nor have small verbal memory loads (Vogel, Woodman, & Luck, 2001). These studies imply that the precaution of employing articulatory suppression may be unnecessary: participants performed no better without articulatory suppression than with it, suggesting that verbal recoding is not the default strategy for visual change detection tasks as typically administered.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, there may be good reasons to believe that this added complexity is justified by the benefits of utilizing both visual and verbal working memory at once. Whereas we have evidence that verbal and visual working memory share resources (e.g., Mate, Allen, & Baqués, 2012;Salmela, Moisala, & Alho, 2014), information that could be stored in either a visual or a verbal format might be more efficiently stored in one or the other. Perhaps for the expressions used, the advantages of processing a pictorial, rather than a verbal, representation justify maintaining a visual format even under the less-than-ideal conditions of Experiments 2 and 3.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Consistent with these findings, there is also neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of visuospatial processes in backward recall (e.g., Hoshi, Oda, Wada, Ito, Yamashita, Oda,… Tamura, 2000; see also Gerton et al, 2004). Although this suggestion may seem to contradict multistore models of memory (e.g., Baddeley, 2000), which assume that information is stored in domain-specific subsystems, there is evidence that recall of verbal information can be improved by the use of visual imagery (e.g., De La Iglesia, Buceta & Campos, 2005), visual representation of a number line (e.g., Dehaene, 1992), or the presentation of digits within familiar visuospatial configurations (Darling, Allen, Havelka, Campbell & Rattray, 2012; see also Mate, Allen & Baques, 2012). It is possible that such visual strategies are employed more so for backward than for forward recall because they can assist with the transformation of the digit sequence.…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%