2016
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00249
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What Klein’s “Semantic Gradient” Does and Does Not Really Show: Decomposing Stroop Interference into Task and Informational Conflict Components

Abstract: The present study suggests that the idea that Stroop interference originates from multiple components may gain theoretically from integrating two independent frameworks. The first framework is represented by the well-known notion of “semantic gradient” of interference and the second one is the distinction between two types of conflict – the task and the informational conflict – giving rise to the interference (MacLeod and MacDonald, 2000; Goldfarb and Henik, 2007). The proposed integration led to the conclusio… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
45
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(53 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
(102 reference statements)
1
45
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The fact that the behavioral manifestation of all three types of conflict can be clearly seen in the Stroop interference observed within the semantic Stroop paradigm administered with vocal responses (Experiment 2) suggests that this paradigm constitutes already a viable avenue of research. At a more general level, it also emphasizes the viability of different multistage accounts and, more specifically, the fact that the two types of multi-stage accounts can easily be integrated in one overarching framework (see e.g., Levin, 2015;Levin & Tzelgov, 2016 for the initial impetus towards this direction).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The fact that the behavioral manifestation of all three types of conflict can be clearly seen in the Stroop interference observed within the semantic Stroop paradigm administered with vocal responses (Experiment 2) suggests that this paradigm constitutes already a viable avenue of research. At a more general level, it also emphasizes the viability of different multistage accounts and, more specifically, the fact that the two types of multi-stage accounts can easily be integrated in one overarching framework (see e.g., Levin, 2015;Levin & Tzelgov, 2016 for the initial impetus towards this direction).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It should be noted that these contrasts are not statistically orthogonal (see e.g.,Levin, 2015;Levin & Tzelgov, 2016 for discussions) but these comparisons were performed to address the associated theoretical issue directly.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Much of the theory and research however has assumed that the interruption, the locus of the Stroop effect, is at the level of responses (Cohen et al, 1990;Roelofs, 2003), and that it is this type of conflict for which control mechanisms monitor (Botvinick et al, 2001). In line with a recent and burgeoning literature (e.g., Parris, 2014;Levin and Tzelgov, 2016;Augustinova et al, 2018;Entel and Tzelgov, 2018;Kalanthroff et al, 2018;Ferrand et al, 2019;Hasshim et al, 2019;Hershman and Henik, 2019;Parris et al, 2019), the contributions to this Research Topic report findings that indicate that there is more than one locus to the Stroop effect.…”
mentioning
confidence: 80%
“…Importantly to the present study, the Stroop effect has been shown to depend on how the irrelevant word names are related to the target colors to which participants respond (Klein, 1964;Levin & Tzelgov, 2016). In particular, Stroop interference is largest when the taskirrelevant word names come from the set of the target colors, but it decreases when the taskirrelevant word names are from outside the set of the target colors.…”
Section: Joint Stroop Taskmentioning
confidence: 39%