2014
DOI: 10.7771/1932-6246.1166
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Wait for it . . . Delaying Instruction Improves Mathematics Problem Solving: A Classroom Study

Abstract: Engaging learners in exploratory problem-solving activities prior to receiving instruction (i.e., explore-instruct approach) has been endorsed as an effective learning approach. However, it remains unclear whether this approach is feasible for elementary-school children in a classroom context. In two experiments, second-graders solved mathematical equivalence problems either before or after receiving brief conceptual instruction. In Experiment 1 (n = 41), the explore-instruct approach was less effective at sup… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
23
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
0
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Finally, the solve-instruct approach could be more effective for students with particular characteristics, such as mastery goal orientation (DeCaro, DeCaro, & Rittle-Johnson, 2015). Other prior research has investigated the solveinstruct approach with elementary-school children in one-on-one tutoring contexts and has yielded mixed results (DeCaro & Rittle-Johnson, 2012;Fyfe, DeCaro, & Rittle-Johnson, 2014;Loehr, Fyfe, & Rittle-Johnson, 2014). Thus, future research is needed to evaluate the potential of a solve-instruct approach for supporting knowledge in elementary-school classrooms, including identifying critical features of such an approach.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Finally, the solve-instruct approach could be more effective for students with particular characteristics, such as mastery goal orientation (DeCaro, DeCaro, & Rittle-Johnson, 2015). Other prior research has investigated the solveinstruct approach with elementary-school children in one-on-one tutoring contexts and has yielded mixed results (DeCaro & Rittle-Johnson, 2012;Fyfe, DeCaro, & Rittle-Johnson, 2014;Loehr, Fyfe, & Rittle-Johnson, 2014). Thus, future research is needed to evaluate the potential of a solve-instruct approach for supporting knowledge in elementary-school classrooms, including identifying critical features of such an approach.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Before instruction, students are given a problem set or problem solving worksheet and are asked to invent a method that solves the problems. This approach has been found to improve conceptual understanding of statistical and mathematical concepts (Loehr, Fyfe, & Rittle-Johnson, 2014;Schwartz & Martin, 2004;Wiedmann, Leach, Rummel, & Wiley, 2012;Wiley, Goldenberg, Jarosz, Wiedmann, & Rummel, 2013). In this paradigm, having knowledge and interest for the cover story could play a bigger role.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We, therefore, agree with calls from mathematics educators to determine what changes can be implemented in US elementary mathematics curricula to foster the development of relational thinking in elementary school (Baroody & Ginsburg, 1983;Carpenter, Levi, Franke, & Zeringue, 2005;Jacobs et al, 2007;McNeil, 2008;Schliemann, Carraher, & Brizuela, 2007;Stephens, Blanton, Knuth, Isler, & Gardiner, in press). Fortunately, there is mounting evidence that even minor differences in curricula, such as teaching the equal sign in concert with inequality symbols , altering the timing of practice and conceptual instruction (Loehr et al, 2014), presenting problems in concrete form (Sherman & Bisanz, 2009), writing addition facts in non-traditional, c = a + b formats (McNeil et al, 2011), and practicing addition facts organized by equivalent sums can increase this relational thinking.…”
Section: Limitations and Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Instead of viewing it relationally as a symbol indicating that two quantities share a common value and are, thus, interchangeable within a mathematical context, they tend to view it operationally as a signal to add up all the numbers and put the total in 'the blank' (Alibali, 1999;Baroody & Ginsburg, 1983;Behr, Erlwanger, & Nichols, 1980;Falkner, Levi, & Carpenter, 1999;Jacobs, Franke, Carpenter, Levi, & Battey, 2007;Li, Ding, Capraro, & Capraro, 2008;Kieran, 1981;Loehr, Fyfe, & Rittle-Johnson, 2014;McNeil, 2005McNeil, , 2008Perry, 1991;Powell & Fuchs, 2010;Renwick, 1932;Weaver, 1973). This is worrisome because this operational way of thinking does not generalize beyond simple arithmetic, and a relational understanding of the equal sign is critical for success in algebra (Falkner et al, 1999;Jacobs et al, 2007;Kieran, 1992;National Research Council, 2001;Steinberg, Sleeman, & Ktorza, 1990).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%