2017
DOI: 10.1002/aur.1915
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visual Motion Prediction and Verbal False Memory Performance in Autistic Children

Abstract: Recent theoretical accounts propose that atypical predictive processing can explain the diverse cognitive and behavioral features associated with autism, and that difficulties in making predictions may be related to reduced contextual processing. In this pre‐registered study, 30 autistic children aged 6–14 years and 30 typically developing children matched in age and non‐verbal IQ completed visual extrapolation and false memory tasks to assess predictive abilities and contextual processing, respectively. In th… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
18
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
3
18
1
Order By: Relevance
“…2b, c; Gordon et al 1991; Flanagan and Beltzner 2000). Although seemingly contradictive of various sensorimotor research (e.g., Mosconi et al 2013; Buckingham et al 2016), this corresponds with a meaningful body of clinical evidence which has shown broad prediction-dependent capabilities to be typical in autistic people (Mostofsky et al 2004; Gidley-Larson et al 2008; Ego et al 2016; Tewolde et al 2018). Findings also align with recent evidence that autistic and neurotypical individuals attend to similar information when presented with visual illusions (Chouinard et al 2018).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 82%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…2b, c; Gordon et al 1991; Flanagan and Beltzner 2000). Although seemingly contradictive of various sensorimotor research (e.g., Mosconi et al 2013; Buckingham et al 2016), this corresponds with a meaningful body of clinical evidence which has shown broad prediction-dependent capabilities to be typical in autistic people (Mostofsky et al 2004; Gidley-Larson et al 2008; Ego et al 2016; Tewolde et al 2018). Findings also align with recent evidence that autistic and neurotypical individuals attend to similar information when presented with visual illusions (Chouinard et al 2018).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 82%
“…However, feedforward atypicalities have not been consistently detected in research (Palmer et al 2017; Tewolde et al 2018). For example, autistic children exhibit typical rates of motor adaptation in various tasks that require, and depend on, broad abilities to utilise ‘top–down’ internal models (Gidley-Larson et al 2008), while prediction-related atypicalities in perception (e.g., global processing; Brosnan et al 2004) do not inevitably transfer onto action or behavior (Palmer et al 2017).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several studies have provided evidence of altered perception of the world in individuals with autism [56][57][58][59][60] and in typically-developing individuals with high autistic traits [61][62][63].…”
Section: Attention Switching Accounts For Context-based Prediction Ofmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…On the other hand, several studies report intact use of priors by individuals with autism. For example, Brown et al [] and Barnes et al [] found no differences in implicit learning in tasks such as contextual cuing in participants with autism, Ego et al [] and Tewolde et al [] found intact motion prediction in autistic children. Croydon et al [] found typical “light‐from‐above” prior in children with autism.…”
Section: Decreased Use Of Prior Knowledge In Perception In Autismmentioning
confidence: 99%