2014
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00437
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Use what you can: storage, abstraction processes, and perceptual adjustments help listeners recognize reduced forms

Abstract: Three eye-tracking experiments tested whether native listeners recognized reduced Dutch words better after having heard the same reduced words, or different reduced words of the same reduction type and whether familiarization with one reduction type helps listeners to deal with another reduction type. In the exposure phase, a segmental reduction group was exposed to /b/-reductions (e.g., minderij instead of binderij, “book binder”) and a syllabic reduction group was exposed to full-vowel deletions (e.g., p'raa… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
(31 reference statements)
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It therefore seems most likely that the effects observed here are due primarily to learning about the reduced forms and not to learning about the quasi-canonical forms. Note that we have also observed adaptation to reductions if the contrast sounds (corresponding to the quasi-canonical stimuli in the present study) were not manipulated (Poellmann, Mitterer, & McQueen, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 49%
“…It therefore seems most likely that the effects observed here are due primarily to learning about the reduced forms and not to learning about the quasi-canonical forms. Note that we have also observed adaptation to reductions if the contrast sounds (corresponding to the quasi-canonical stimuli in the present study) were not manipulated (Poellmann, Mitterer, & McQueen, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 49%
“…Such a modulation of processing has been demonstrated with other aspects of context situations (Brouwer, Mitterer, & Huettig, 2012;McQueen & Huettig, 2012;Poellmann, Mitterer, & McQueen, 2014). That is, when listening to clear speech, listeners give special weight to segments in word onset to modulate lexical access and identify the word that is being said.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…That is, when listening to clear speech, listeners give special weight to segments in word onset to modulate lexical access and identify the word that is being said. However, if context information suggests that the segments in the word onset may be less reliable than expected because the context is partially masked by radio noise (McQueen & Huettig, 2012) or contains segmental reductions in casual speech (Brouwer et al, 2012; Poellmann et al, 2014), then listeners reduce their reliance on word-initial segments in spoken-word recognition. Segmental mismatches in word-onset position thus cause less bottom-up inhibition in accessing acoustically similar words.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…momentary degree of overall support for this word as a lexical candidate (Allopenna et al, 1998). The visual world paradigm with pictures or with printed words can capture the time course of phonological (segmental) competition among lexical candidates (e.g., Huettig & McQueen, 2007;McQueen & Viebahn, 2007;Poellmann, Mitterer, & McQueen, 2014;Reinisch, Jesse, & McQueen, 2011a;Salverda & Tanenhaus, 2010) and is sensitive to how prosodic information alters online spoken-word recognition in various languages (e.g., Ito & Speer, 2008;Reinisch et al, 2010;Salverda et al, 2003Salverda et al, , 2007. Most relevant to the present study, in a visual world paradigm with printed words, Dutch listeners used suprasegmental information associated with the primary-stressed syllable of accentuated words to speed up spoken-word recognition and to resolve lexical competition before the target words became segmentally unique (Reinisch et al, 2010).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%