2011
DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02019.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Use of indirect addition in adults’ mental subtraction in the number domain up to 1,000

Abstract: This study examined adults' use of indirect addition and direct subtraction strategies on multi-digit subtractions in the number domain up to 1,000. Seventy students who differed in their level of arithmetic ability solved multi-digit subtractions in one choice and two no-choice conditions. Against the background of recent findings in elementary subtraction, we manipulated the size of the subtrahend compared to the difference and only selected items with large distances between these two integers. Results reve… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
31
2

Year Published

2011
2011
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
5
31
2
Order By: Relevance
“…First, research evidence shows that individuals have a preference for using the rule of addition to solve numerical problems (e.g., solving a simple subtraction problem by calculating how much should be added to the smaller number to arrive at the larger number). (45) In addition, Brown and Morley (2007) have found that individuals' estimates of perceived risk are not necessarily numerically specific, but are typically fuzzy and span a range of possible estimates. (36) Hence, many of our participants may have interpreted our scenarios as numerical problems and, consequently, employed an addition rule for convenience and to alleviate task complexity.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, research evidence shows that individuals have a preference for using the rule of addition to solve numerical problems (e.g., solving a simple subtraction problem by calculating how much should be added to the smaller number to arrive at the larger number). (45) In addition, Brown and Morley (2007) have found that individuals' estimates of perceived risk are not necessarily numerically specific, but are typically fuzzy and span a range of possible estimates. (36) Hence, many of our participants may have interpreted our scenarios as numerical problems and, consequently, employed an addition rule for convenience and to alleviate task complexity.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Following a choice/no-choice design, Torbeyns, Ghesquière, et al (2009) and Torbeyns, De Smedt, Peters, Ghesquière, and Verschaffel (2011) empirically addressed adults' strategy use on multi-digit subtraction problems. In three subsequent studies, Torbeyns et al (2009Torbeyns et al ( , 2011 offered adults different types of multi-digit subtraction problems with numbers up to 1,000 in one choice and two no-choice conditions. In the choice condition, participants had to choose between direct subtraction and subtraction by addition.…”
Section: Previous Studies On Direct Subtraction Versus Subtraction Bymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While previous studies have shown that adults use the subtraction by addition strategy frequently, efficiently (i.e., fast and accurately), and flexibly (i.e., mainly, but not exclusively on problems with a relatively large subtrahend) (Peters, De Smedt, Torbeyns, Ghesquière, & Verschaffel, ,; Torbeyns, De Smedt, Peters, Ghesquière, & Verschaffel, ; Torbeyns, Ghesquière, et al ., ), well‐documented evidence on primary school children's self‐reported use of the subtraction by addition strategy when mentally solving two‐digit subtraction problems is limited (Blöte et al ., ; De Smedt et al ., ; Selter, ; Torbeyns et al ., ). For example, Torbeyns et al .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…While previous studies have shown that adults use the subtraction by addition strategy frequently, efficiently (i.e., fast and accurately), and flexibly (i.e., mainly, but not exclusively on problems with a relatively large subtrahend) (Peters, De Smedt, Torbeyns, Ghesquière, & Verschaffel, 2010a,b;Torbeyns, De Smedt, Peters, Ghesquière, & Verschaffel, 2011;, well-documented evidence on primary school children's self-reported use of the subtraction by addition strategy when mentally solving two-digit subtraction problems is limited (Blöte et al, 2000;De Smedt 1 People can also use a third strategy, the so-called indirect subtraction strategy in which they determine how much needs to be subtracted from the minuend to get to the subtrahend (e.g., 75 À 43 = . by 75 À 30 = 45 and 45 À 2 = 43; so the answer is 30 + 2 = 32) (De Corte & Verschaffel, 1987).…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%