2019
DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x18002327
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Two sources of bias affecting the evaluation of autistic communication

Abstract: We support Jaswal & Akhtar's interrogation of social motivational accounts of autism and discuss two sources of bias that contribute to how others construe autistic people's communications: (1) an experience-based bias that limits our ability to discern the speaker's action as communicative and (2) a prejudice against the credibility of certain speakers that limits a listener's willingness to believe their testimony.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
2

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…People often generalize inappropriately from their own behaviors to determine what counts as social or communicative behavior in others; this is an experience-based bias that constrains the ability to view others' unconventional or idiosyncratic behaviors as attempts to communicate and connect (Li & Koenig, 2019). But we should keep in mind that communication and connection are reciprocal processes; this seems obvious but tends to be ignored when one member of a dyad is autistic (Dinishak & Akhtar, 2013;Gernsbacher, 2006;Heasman & Gillespie, 2018;Milton, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…People often generalize inappropriately from their own behaviors to determine what counts as social or communicative behavior in others; this is an experience-based bias that constrains the ability to view others' unconventional or idiosyncratic behaviors as attempts to communicate and connect (Li & Koenig, 2019). But we should keep in mind that communication and connection are reciprocal processes; this seems obvious but tends to be ignored when one member of a dyad is autistic (Dinishak & Akhtar, 2013;Gernsbacher, 2006;Heasman & Gillespie, 2018;Milton, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In several studies, nonautistic people misperceived the social signals of autistic people (for a review, see Milton, Heasman, & Sheppard, 2018). Thus, researchers need to not only look beyond typical indicators of sociality, but also recognize that their appraisals of others' sociality (including prejudices and inaccurate assumptions; Li & Koenig, 2019) can have cascading negative effects (Akhtar, Jaswal, Dinishak, & Stephan, 2016) and even become self-fulfilling prophecies (Jaswal & Akhtar, 2019a). If developmentalists are truly interested in all of human development, it is imperative that we broaden what counts as social behavior.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When it comes to neurotypicals, Hacking writes, "there has been a language for the intentions, desires, and emotions of other people for all of historical time " (2009b, 56). While this language for describing experience "crafted by and for neurotypicals" (56) goes back to our distant ancestors, it has evolved and will keep 1 E.g., Dohmen 2016;Hens, Robeyns, and Schaubroeck 2018;Li and Koenig 2019;Legault, Bourdon, and Poirier 2019. 2 Hacking himself does not explore the implications of this work for understanding the epistemic injustice autists suffer.…”
Section: Language For Describing Experiences and Why It's Been Missing For Autistic Experiencementioning
confidence: 99%