2009
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.09.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Two are better than one: Comparison influences infants’ visual recognition memory

Abstract: Despite a large literature on infants' memory for visually presented stimuli, the processes underlying visual memory are not well understood. Two studies with 4-month-old infants (N = 60) examined the effects of providing opportunities for comparison of items on infants' memory for those items. Experiment 1 revealed that 4-month-old infants failed to show evidence of memory for an item presented during familiarization in a standard task (i.e., when only one item was presented during familiarization). In Experi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
23
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
(57 reference statements)
3
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…One possibility is that co-occurrences of visual and spatial information are supporting the individuation of the objects, allowing two different sounds to be associated with two memory representations. Another possibility is that the objects are always individuated, even with minimal cues, but co-occurrences between multiple cues make the representations more robust in memory (e.g., Feigenson, 2005;Oakes, Kovack-Lesh, & Horst, 2009). In each case, however, infants are encoding visual features in indexing situations and are using this information to predict the appearance of an event.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One possibility is that co-occurrences of visual and spatial information are supporting the individuation of the objects, allowing two different sounds to be associated with two memory representations. Another possibility is that the objects are always individuated, even with minimal cues, but co-occurrences between multiple cues make the representations more robust in memory (e.g., Feigenson, 2005;Oakes, Kovack-Lesh, & Horst, 2009). In each case, however, infants are encoding visual features in indexing situations and are using this information to predict the appearance of an event.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…” (unlabeled). The experimenter explained to the caregiver that they should encourage their child to interact with both toys for an approximately equal amount of time, and that their child should be allowed to play with both toys at the same time (to encourage comparison, which promotes encoding; Gentner & Namy, 1999; Oakes, Kovack‐Lesh, & Horst, 2009). Infants heard the label approximately twice every 15 seconds.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Here too, studies across a wide range of topics suggest that too many competing items can disrupt learning (e.g., in wordlearning [Horst, Scott, & Pollard, 2010]; science learning in museum settings [Allen & Gutwill, 2004], target identification in rapid presentations [Shapiro, Raymond, & Arnell, 1997], working memory [Carroll et al, 2010], and long-term memory [Axmacher, Haupt, Cohen, Elgar, & Fell, 2009]). On the other hand, the presence of multiple exemplars has also been shown to aid infants and children in forming categories (Graham, Namy, Gentner, & Meagher, 2010;Quinn & Tanaka, 2007;Waxman, Chambers, Yntema, & Gelman, 1989), remembering hidden objects (Oakes, Kovack-Lesh, & Horst, 2009), and in learning "deeper" relational information (Gentner & Namy, 1999). Thus, the role of competing options in helping or hindering learning remains an open question.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%