2004
DOI: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00742.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Trust in Testimony: Children's Use of True and False Statements

Abstract: The extent to which young children monitor and use the truth of assertions to gauge the reliability of subsequent testimony was examined. Three- and 4-year-old children were presented with two informants, an accurate labeler and an inaccurate labeler. They were then invited to learn names for novel objects from these informants. The children correctly monitored and identified the informants on the basis of the truth of their prior labeling. Furthermore, children who explicitly identified the unreliable or reli… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

29
539
3
4

Year Published

2007
2007
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 541 publications
(575 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
29
539
3
4
Order By: Relevance
“…To manage this risk, children need ways of predicting the likely truth of what they are told. One way children do this is by tracking the speaker's history of accuracy: 4-and 5-year-olds learn new information from a speaker with a history of accuracy rather than from one with a history of inaccuracy (Birch, Vauthier, & Bloom, in press;Jaswal & Neely, 2006;Koenig, Clément, & Harris, 2004;Koenig & Harris, 2005;Scofield & Behrend, 2008). In a typical task, the child participant observes two speakers name several familiar objects, one of whom consistently gives accurate names, while the other gives inaccurate names for no apparent reason, thereby rendering themselves untrustworthy as informants.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To manage this risk, children need ways of predicting the likely truth of what they are told. One way children do this is by tracking the speaker's history of accuracy: 4-and 5-year-olds learn new information from a speaker with a history of accuracy rather than from one with a history of inaccuracy (Birch, Vauthier, & Bloom, in press;Jaswal & Neely, 2006;Koenig, Clément, & Harris, 2004;Koenig & Harris, 2005;Scofield & Behrend, 2008). In a typical task, the child participant observes two speakers name several familiar objects, one of whom consistently gives accurate names, while the other gives inaccurate names for no apparent reason, thereby rendering themselves untrustworthy as informants.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research on how children determine fair procedures for offering rewards focuses on the needs of the contestants in contests, or the requirements for winning the contests (e.g., skill versus luck), not on the qualifications of the judges themselves (e.g., Sigelman & Waitzman, 1991; Thorkidsen & White-McNulty, 2002). However, related research examining how children evaluate information suggests that young children recognize the importance of informed testimony and judgments: They distinguish knowledgeable speakers from ignorant ones when learning new words (Jaswal & Neely, 2006;Koenig, Clement, & Harris, 2004;Sabbagh & Baldwin, 2001) and when dealing with misinformation (Welch-Ross, 1999). Four-year-olds understand that sometimes people do not have enough knowledge to provide accurate statements (Sodian, 1988;Wimmer & Perner, 1983), and that some people are more knowledgeable than others for a given topic (Lutz & Keil, 2002).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, according to David Hume, a good judge meets the following description: "strong sense, united to delicate sentiment, improved by practice, perfected by comparison, and cleared of all prejudice, can alone entitle critics to this valuable character," (Hume, 1757). As mentioned in the introduction, previous research has found that even young children prefer listening to and learning from speakers who meet some of these criteria, such as being knowledgeable and perceptually capable (e.g., Jaswal & Neely, 2006;Koenig, Clement, & Harris, 2004;Sabbagh & Baldwin, 2001). Other research with adults finds that they are concerned about judgments that seem skewed by bias (e.g., Miller & Ratner, 1998).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Elementary forms of cognitive grasping of these principles might be available at early stages in the development of human cognition (e.g., Baillargeon, 2001;Spelke, 1990), with a more explicit understanding of them emerging in adult sensorimotor skills (Ballard, Hayhoe, Pook, & Rao, 1997;Gibson, 1979;Noë, 2004) and adult 'folk physics' (McCloskey, 1983;McCloskey, Caramazza, & Green, 1980). On the other hand, humans can also interact with and reason about existing, dead or fictional animated 6 and intentional agents-i.e., animated creatures to whom we ascribe intentional states such as beliefs, desires and intentions (e.g., Astuti, 2001;Bloom, 2002Bloom, , 2004Bonatti, Frot, Zangl, & Mehler, 2002;Dennett, 1971Dennett, , 1987Koenig, Clément, & Harris, 2004;Malle, Moses, & Baldwin, 2001;Puccetti, 1968). In order to account for the behavior of such agents we need to add principles not reducible to those used to track physical objects.…”
Section: Components Of Singular Reference and Cognitionmentioning
confidence: 99%