2019
DOI: 10.1007/s10071-019-01257-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Transitive inference in pigeons may result from differential tendencies to reject the test stimuli acquired during training

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
1

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
0
8
1
Order By: Relevance
“…These findings suggest that complexity can be added to the task by manipulating the properties of stimuli (colors vs. flags) and the amount of stimuli. However, Zentall et al (2019) did not find differences in the amount of sessions to reach the criterion when comparing colors and flags across phases, whereas we found differences in the amount of errors to reach the criterion. It is possible that probabilistic reinforcement and the complexity of stimuli have different effects on TI procedures.…”
Section: Other Experiments Involving Discriminationcontrasting
confidence: 96%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These findings suggest that complexity can be added to the task by manipulating the properties of stimuli (colors vs. flags) and the amount of stimuli. However, Zentall et al (2019) did not find differences in the amount of sessions to reach the criterion when comparing colors and flags across phases, whereas we found differences in the amount of errors to reach the criterion. It is possible that probabilistic reinforcement and the complexity of stimuli have different effects on TI procedures.…”
Section: Other Experiments Involving Discriminationcontrasting
confidence: 96%
“…Further research showed the first evidence of TI in monkeys (McGonigle and Chalmers, 1977 ). After those initial findings, TI has been explored in several organisms, such as humans (Gillan, 1981 ; Galizio et al, 2017 ), children (Mou et al, 2013 ), monkeys (Jensen et al, 2019a ), crows (Lazareva et al, 2004 ), pigeons (Lazareva and Wasserman, 2012 ; Zentall et al, 2019 ), wasps (Tibbetts et al, 2019 ), and fish (Grosenick et al, 2007 ). More recent approaches have modeled TI in neural networks (Frank et al, 2003 ; Jensen et al, 2019b ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…E). Therefore, subjects could select the transitively correct item on novel test pairs (e.g., B vs. D) by selecting the item with the higher associative value (e.g., B), creating an appearance of an inferential process while instead relying on associative mechanisms (see also Zentall et al, 2019).…”
Section: Can Ti Performance Be Explained Solely Bymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Through these mechanisms, the relative associative values of the stimuli in the TI set could fall out such that they produce the same order as the transitively derived order of the stimuli (A > B > C > D > E). Therefore, subjects could select the transitively correct item on novel test pairs (e.g., B vs. D) by selecting the item with the higher associative value (e.g., B), creating an appearance of an inferential process while instead relying on associative mechanisms (see also Zentall et al, 2019).…”
Section: Can Ti Performance Be Explained Solely By Associative Proces...mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…More recent studies in TI have not reported such as dropout ratio in pigeons (five out of seven) during premise pair C + D- ( Lazareva and Wasserman, 2006 , 2012 ; Camarena et al, 2018 ; Zentall et al, 2019 ), but those procedures used probabilities of 0 and 1, which makes the discrimination easier than the one employed in our experiment. Other manipulations reported dropouts in subjects’ performance, for example, von Fersen et al (1991) used six pigeons exposed to all premise pairs in experiment one, but before the test, two subjects were excluded because they did not reach the 60% criterion.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 64%