2018
DOI: 10.3758/s13415-018-0571-y
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Transient and sustained incentive effects on electrophysiological indices of cognitive control in younger and older adults

Abstract: Preparing for upcoming events, separating task-relevant from task-irrelevant information and efficiently responding to stimuli all require cognitive control. The adaptive recruitment of cognitive control depends on activity in the dopaminergic reward system as well as the frontoparietal control network. In healthy aging, dopaminergic neuromodulation is reduced, resulting in altered incentive-based recruitment of control mechanisms. In the present study, younger adults (18-28 years) and healthy older adults (66… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
5

Citation Types

7
41
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(49 citation statements)
references
References 105 publications
(138 reference statements)
7
41
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Second, many of these real-world situations rely heavily on working memory, and age differences in working memory are both large and a topic of central interest in both theoretical work and empirical studies of cognition and performance (see Park and Festini, 2017 for a recent review). However, most performance-incentive studies have focused on measures related to attention and cognitive control ( Di Rosa et al, 2015 ; Schmitt et al, 2015 , 2017 ; Williams et al, 2017 , 2018 ; Yee et al, 2019 ), and only a handful have compared young and older adults. As noted above, there have also been a number of reinforcement learning and episodic memory studies focusing more on the ability to learn reward/loss associations or prioritize high vs. low reward items (e.g., Castel et al, 2002 ; Cohen et al, 2016 ), as well as studies on incentivized episodic memory encoding (e.g., Spaniol et al, 2014 ; Geddes et al, 2018 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Second, many of these real-world situations rely heavily on working memory, and age differences in working memory are both large and a topic of central interest in both theoretical work and empirical studies of cognition and performance (see Park and Festini, 2017 for a recent review). However, most performance-incentive studies have focused on measures related to attention and cognitive control ( Di Rosa et al, 2015 ; Schmitt et al, 2015 , 2017 ; Williams et al, 2017 , 2018 ; Yee et al, 2019 ), and only a handful have compared young and older adults. As noted above, there have also been a number of reinforcement learning and episodic memory studies focusing more on the ability to learn reward/loss associations or prioritize high vs. low reward items (e.g., Castel et al, 2002 ; Cohen et al, 2016 ), as well as studies on incentivized episodic memory encoding (e.g., Spaniol et al, 2014 ; Geddes et al, 2018 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It does not seem to straightforwardly apply to the motivation-cognitive performance questions of interest here. Indeed, those studies that have examined the effects of loss incentive on older adults’ response to cognitive demands are relatively consistent in showing that older adults have either an equivalent or reduced response to loss incentive compared to young adults and/or to positive incentive (e.g., Bagurdes et al, 2008 ; Di Rosa et al, 2015 ; Schmitt et al, 2015 , 2017 ; Pachur et al, 2017 ; Williams et al, 2017 , 2018 ). Thus, while we note that the motivational shift hypothesis might superficially appear to predict larger performance improvements, greater motivation, and increased metacognitive accuracy for older adults in the loss condition, we do not consider it likely to apply to the current study.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, most studies have focused on measures related to attention and cognitive control (Di Rosa, Schiff, Cagnolati, & Mapelli, 2015;Schmitt, Ferdinand, & Kray, 2015;Schmitt, Kray, & Ferdinand, 2017;Williams, Biel, Dyson, & Spaniol, 2017;Williams, Kudus, Dyson, & Spaniol, 2018;Yee, Adams, Beck, & Braver, 2019), and only a handful have compared young and older adults. To our knowledge, despite well-established and large age differences in working memory, and the fundamental role working memory is thought to play in performance in many domains, only one study has examined the effects of incentive on working memory in younger and older adults (Thurm, Zink, & Li, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It does not appear to apply to the motivation-cognition interactions of interest here. Indeed, although there are only a few studies examining the effects of loss incentive on older adults' response to cognitive demands, they are relatively consistent in showing that older adults have either an equivalent or reduced response to loss incentive compared to young adults (e.g., Di Rosa, Schiff, Cagnolati, & Mapelli, 2015;Pachur, Mata, & Hertwig, 2017;Schmitt et al, 2015Schmitt et al, , 2017Williams, Biel, Dyson, & Spaniol, 2017;Williams, Kudus, Dyson, & Spaniol, 2018)). Thus, while we note that the motivational shift hypothesis might superficially appear to predict larger performance improvements, greater motivation, and increased metacognitive accuracy for older adults in the loss condition, we do not consider it likely to apply to the current study.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation