2005
DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2004.11.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Towards a general theory of driver behaviour

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

24
401
1
10

Year Published

2009
2009
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 639 publications
(436 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
24
401
1
10
Order By: Relevance
“…This study also only focused on one part of Fuller's [1] model, how demand can affect behaviour, the second part of the model suggests that competence may also have an effect.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This study also only focused on one part of Fuller's [1] model, how demand can affect behaviour, the second part of the model suggests that competence may also have an effect.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Fuller's [1] model suggests that drivers' behaviour is regulated by a desire to maintain an acceptable level of task difficulty which varies based on two factors: driver competence and task demand. The current study manipulated task demand in order to test whether this theory can also help explain drivers' phone use behaviour.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…RAT differs from RHT in that it specifies that individuals maintain a target range of a feeling of risk, rather than a single target level of crash risk put forward by RHT. Also RAT states that this target range is a lot more flexible and open to change than the target level of risk within RHT, which was seen as quite fixed and stable (Fuller & Santos, 2002;Fuller, 2005;Wilde, 1988). Within the theoretical underpinnings of RAT, Fuller is quite clear that "the effects of risk on decision making are not binary" and that "task difficulty and feelings of risk are continuously present variables which inform driver decisions" (Fuller et al, 2008, p. 31).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The psychological mechanisms behind BA are yet to be elucidated, but it has been postulated that drivers exhibit a trade-off between two conflicting motivations, namely arriving at a destination in time (efficiency) versus avoiding dangerous situations (safety), and whereby the driver's level of subjective risk (Näätänen & Summala, 1974;Wilde, 2013;Wilde, 1998), task difficulty (Fuller, 2005), or time/safety margins (Gibson & Crooks, 1938;van Winsum, de Waard, & Brookhuis, 1999) are important homeostatic variables. Accordingly, drivers adopt a higher speed or a shorter headway when the driving task becomes easier, less risky, or less temporally demanding due to a change in the road-vehicle-driver system, such as improved environmental conditions (e.g., increased speed that occurs when adding road lighting; Assum et al, 1999) or increased controllability of the car itself (e.g., increased speed and shorter headway time adopted when driving with an adaptive cruise control; Dragutinovic et al, 2005).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%