1978
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1978.29-191
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

TIME‐ALLOCATION MATCHING BETWEEN PUNISHING SITUATIONS1

Abstract: In the presence and absence of white noise, response-independent aversive events were delivered to rats according to several variable-time electric-shock schedules. The animals could switch from the noise component to the no-noise component and vice versa by making a single lever-press response. If the schedule in one component was not in operation when the animal was in the other component, the proportion of time allocated to one component equalled or matched the proportion of obtained punishers in the other … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

6
23
0

Year Published

1978
1978
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
6
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…A schedule of punishment superimposed on one response alternative of a concurrent schedule shifts the distribution of behavior toward the alternative associated with no punishment even when there is a net loss of reinforcement. This shift has been reported with animals (Deluty & Church, 1978;de Villiers, 1980;Farley & Fantino, 1978;Wojnicki & Barrett, 1993) and humans (e.g., Bradshaw, Szabadi, & Bevan, 1979;Carlton, Siegel, Murphee, & Cook, 1981;Critchfield, Paletz, & MacAleese, 2003;Katz, 1973;Rasmussen & Newland, 2008). A global shift in preference is captured by the bias parameter (log k) in the generalized matching equation (Carlton et al, 1981;Rasmussen & Newland, 2008).…”
supporting
confidence: 56%
“…A schedule of punishment superimposed on one response alternative of a concurrent schedule shifts the distribution of behavior toward the alternative associated with no punishment even when there is a net loss of reinforcement. This shift has been reported with animals (Deluty & Church, 1978;de Villiers, 1980;Farley & Fantino, 1978;Wojnicki & Barrett, 1993) and humans (e.g., Bradshaw, Szabadi, & Bevan, 1979;Carlton, Siegel, Murphee, & Cook, 1981;Critchfield, Paletz, & MacAleese, 2003;Katz, 1973;Rasmussen & Newland, 2008). A global shift in preference is captured by the bias parameter (log k) in the generalized matching equation (Carlton et al, 1981;Rasmussen & Newland, 2008).…”
supporting
confidence: 56%
“…Second, with those responses measured with some kind of discrete operandum (e.g., mechanical switch), punished responding generally goes unmeasured other than by indicating a decrease in responding. Some researchers argued that punishment functions as a negative reinforcer for other responses (e.g., Deluty, 1976;Deluty & Church, 1978) -the so-called additive theory of punishment -whereas others argued that punishment directly reduces responding that has produced a punisher (e.g., Critchfield et al, 2003;de Villiers, 1980;Farley, 1980)-the so-called subtractive theory of punishment. Klapes, Riley, and McDowell (2018) recently argued that neither theory describes the data adequately.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…If we make the reasonable assumption that reinforcement of only one alternative produces exclusive preference for that alternative-i.e., that when proportion of reinforcement equals 1.0 or 0.0, so too does proportion of behaviorthen we see that deviations from matching in the coordinates of Figure 1 & Church, 1978;Menlove, Moffitt, & Shimp, 1973;Myers & Myers, 1977;Poling, 1978). Equation 1 provides a sensible alternative.…”
mentioning
confidence: 80%