The Probabilistic Mind: 2008
DOI: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199216093.003.0018
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Through the looking glass: a dynamic lens model approach to multiple cue probability learning

Abstract: Despite what the somewhat technical name might suggest, multiple cue probability learning (MCPL) problems are commonly encountered in daily life. For instance, we may have to judge whether it will rain from cues such as temperature, humidity, and the time of year. Or, we may have to judge whether someone is telling the truth from cues such as pitch of voice, level of eye contact, and rate of eye blinks. While informative, these cues are not perfect predictors. How do we learn to solve such problems? How do we … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
3
1
1

Relationship

2
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Another related paradigm is multiple cue probability learning (MCPL; Kruschke & Johansen, 1999; Speekenbrink & Shanks, 2008) in which participants are shown an array of cues that are probabilistically related to an outcome and have to learn the underlying function mapping the cues’ features to expected outcomes. Especially when the outcome is a categorical variable, such as in the well-known weather prediction task (Gluck, Shohamy, & Myers, 2002; Speekenbrink, Channon, & Shanks, 2008), making a prediction is structurally similar to a decision between multiple arms (possible predictions) that are rewarded (correct prediction) or not (incorrect prediction).…”
Section: Cmabsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Another related paradigm is multiple cue probability learning (MCPL; Kruschke & Johansen, 1999; Speekenbrink & Shanks, 2008) in which participants are shown an array of cues that are probabilistically related to an outcome and have to learn the underlying function mapping the cues’ features to expected outcomes. Especially when the outcome is a categorical variable, such as in the well-known weather prediction task (Gluck, Shohamy, & Myers, 2002; Speekenbrink, Channon, & Shanks, 2008), making a prediction is structurally similar to a decision between multiple arms (possible predictions) that are rewarded (correct prediction) or not (incorrect prediction).…”
Section: Cmabsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To alleviate these problems, the change process should be explicitly incorporated in the model. We have proposed to do this in a state–space model formalism (Speekenbrink & Shanks, 2008). The resulting dynamic lens model (DLM) consists of two dynamic linear models (e.g., West & Harrison, 1997).…”
Section: Dynamic Lens Model Analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our task involved only deterministic rules, but included rules of different complexity, focusing on high-order dependencies between elements in the pattern. We extended ideas of feature-based learning (6,(11)(12)(13) to present a reinforcement learning-like family of maximum entropy-based models to describe how individuals learn different high-order classification rules. We found that these models capture individual behavior to a high degree of accuracy, and can also predict individual behavior.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%