1997
DOI: 10.1068/p261147
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Use of Image Blur as a Depth Cue

Abstract: Images of three-dimensional scenes inevitably contain regions that are spatially blurred by differing amounts, owing to depth-of-focus limitations in the imaging apparatus. Recent perceptual data indicate that this blur variation acts as an effective cue to depth: if one image region contains sharply focused texture, and another contains blurred texture, then the two regions may be perceived at different depths, even in the absence of other depth cues. Calculations based on the optical properties of the human … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
53
1

Year Published

2002
2002
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
4
4
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 81 publications
(59 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
5
53
1
Order By: Relevance
“…As reference blur increases beyond the value of the intrinsic space constant, the contribution of the latter diminishes. The minimum of the dipper function corresponds to the value of the intrinsic blur space constant (Watt 1988;Mather 1997). Table 2 shows the value of the Weber fraction and the intrinsic blur space constant for the best-fitting function in each experiment, along with the coefficient of determination.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…As reference blur increases beyond the value of the intrinsic space constant, the contribution of the latter diminishes. The minimum of the dipper function corresponds to the value of the intrinsic blur space constant (Watt 1988;Mather 1997). Table 2 shows the value of the Weber fraction and the intrinsic blur space constant for the best-fitting function in each experiment, along with the coefficient of determination.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Pentland (1987) identified image blur as a more general source of depth information both in machine vision and in human vision. However, only recently has blur-mediated depth perception become a matter for investigation in psychophysical studies (Marshall et al 1996;Mather 1996Mather , 1997O'Shea et al 1997;Mather and Smith 2000). Owing to the depth-of-focus limitations of the eye's optics, retinal images of objects nearer or farther than the plane of fixation are blurred by an amount that depends on their relative distance from the fixation plane.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some of these cues result from binocularity such as ocular vergence angle (Collewijn & Erkelens, 1990;Mon-Williams, Tresilian, & Roberts, 2000;Richard & Miller, 1969;Ritter, 1977;Viguier, Clement, & Trotter, 2001) and retinal disparity (Bishop, 1989;Mayhew & Longuet-Higgins, 1982), whilst monocular cues such as retinal blurring (Mather, 1997;O'Shea, Govan, & Sekuler, 1997), ocular accommodation (MonWilliams & Tresilian, 1999, 2000, as well as the surface texture, contrast and shading of fixated objects are also influential (Gonzalez & Perez, 1998;Johnston, 1991;Johnston, Cumming, & Parker, 1993;O'Shea, Blackburn, & Ono, 1994). …”
Section: General Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The depth-aware video enhancement makes the video more vivid and more realistic. Using the characteristics of human visual perception in [11][12] [13]. Three cues, contrast, saturation, and edge, are used to enhance the depth perception according to relative depth range.…”
Section: Depth-aware 2-d Video Enhancementmentioning
confidence: 99%