1997
DOI: 10.1006/cogp.1997.0651
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Tree of Life: Universal and Cultural Features of Folkbiological Taxonomies and Inductions

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

17
250
1
2

Year Published

2002
2002
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
6
4

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 319 publications
(270 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
(48 reference statements)
17
250
1
2
Order By: Relevance
“…These differences can be explored by (i) comparing first and second factor scores of each individual and (iii) analyzing patterns of residual agreement. Residual agreement is calculated by subtracting predicted agreement from the observed agreement (24)(25)(26). To the extent that within-group residual agreement is larger than cross-group residual agreement, one has evidence of reliable group differences.…”
Section: Mental Models Of Nature Among Fish Expertsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These differences can be explored by (i) comparing first and second factor scores of each individual and (iii) analyzing patterns of residual agreement. Residual agreement is calculated by subtracting predicted agreement from the observed agreement (24)(25)(26). To the extent that within-group residual agreement is larger than cross-group residual agreement, one has evidence of reliable group differences.…”
Section: Mental Models Of Nature Among Fish Expertsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Infants also possess more meaningful or conceptual categories (Mandler, 1992), such as the distinction between kinds of objects (animate vs. inanimate or natural kinds vs. artifacts) or kinds of mechanical interactions (containment vs. support or inert vs. self-propelled), that allow them to interpret and make predictions about the outcome of physical and social events (e.g., Baillargeon, 1998Baillargeon, , 2004Leslie, 1994;Meltzoff & Moore, 1995;Premack, 1990;Spelke & Woodward, 1995). In adults, these are often referred to as natural categories (Ross & Murphy, 1999) and a great deal of effort has been placed on understanding how adults and children use these categories to draw inferences about the properties an object will possess or the purpose/function of an object (Gelman & Coley, 1990;Gelman & Koenig, 2003;Gelman & Markman, 1986;Kalish & Gelman, 1992;Lopez, Atran, Coley, Medin, & Smith, 1997;Malt, Ross, & Murphy, 1995;Malt & Smith, 1984;Markman, 1989;Rosch, Mervis, Gray, Johnson, & Boyes-Braem, 1976;Ross & Murphy, 1999). These kinds of categories are evident very early in development, are relatively stable, and are gradually shaped over time by infants' and children's everyday experiences in the physical and social world.…”
Section: What Kinds Of Categories Are These?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Western adults who have scarce knowledge of folkbiology exhibit more diversity than Western experts (e.g., Bailenson, Shum, Atran, Medin, & Coley, 2002;Osherson et al, 1990;Proffitt, Coley, & Medin, 2000). Itza Mayas exhibit diversity when reasoning about abstract logic problems, but not when reasoning about categories they are familiar with like trees or plants (López, Atran, Coley, Medin, & Smith, 1997).…”
Section: Towards a Unified Accountmentioning
confidence: 99%