2008
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2008.02.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Color–function categories that prime infants to use color information in an object individuation task

Abstract: There is evidence for developmental hierarchies in the type of information to which infants attend when reasoning about objects. Investigators have questioned the origin of these hierarchies and how infants come to identify new sources of information when reasoning about objects. The goal of the present experiments was to shed light on this debate by identifying conditions under which infants' sensitivity to color information, which is slow to emerge, could be enhanced in an object individuation task. The outc… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
35
0
1

Year Published

2011
2011
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 30 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 127 publications
1
35
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…While these studies interestingly demonstrate that children do not preferentially attend to color early in development, our results suggest that children nonetheless can abstract color from an early age if color is highlighted linguistically. In fact, in studies where color properties are highlighted, evidence of color abstraction has been found in infants as young as five to twelve months of age (Catherwood, Crassini & Freiberg, 1989;Waxman, 2007;Wilcox, Woods & Chapa, 2008;Wilcox, 1999).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While these studies interestingly demonstrate that children do not preferentially attend to color early in development, our results suggest that children nonetheless can abstract color from an early age if color is highlighted linguistically. In fact, in studies where color properties are highlighted, evidence of color abstraction has been found in infants as young as five to twelve months of age (Catherwood, Crassini & Freiberg, 1989;Waxman, 2007;Wilcox, Woods & Chapa, 2008;Wilcox, 1999).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For these older infants, some additional evidence suggests that infants used the featural differences between objects to set up contrasting kind representations and that these kind representations are critical to individuation (Bonatti, Frot, Zangl, & Mehler, 2002;Xu, Carey, & Quint, 2004;Xu, Cote, & Baker, 2005). However, when the task is made simpler (e.g., Wilcox & Baillargeon, 1998b;Wilcox & Chapa, 2002;Wilcox & Schweinle, 2002) or when given additional experience with objects' features (Wilcox, Woods, & Chapa, 2008;Wilcox, Woods, Chapa, & McCurry, 2007), infants much younger than 12 months also appear to use featural cues to individuate and can do so with objects that do not clearly contrast in kind membership (e.g., ball vs. box). Taken together, these studies of infants' object individuation abilities have revealed which object features infants can use for individuation as well as which experimental paradigms make individuation more or less difficult (e.g., Bonatti et al, 2002;Krojgaard, 2007;Surian & Caldi, 2010;Wilcox & Baillargeon, 1998a, 1998bXu et al, 2005).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Being able to sit up and to crawl increases the opportunities for infants to explore objects and thus enhances their ability to process and reason about objects (e.g., Soska, Adolph, & Johnson, 2010). In addition to naturally occurring experience, deliberate manipulation of experiences at home or in the laboratory enhances infants' perception of and performance in dynamic events (e.g., Amso & Johnson, 2006Barrett, Davis, & Needham, 2007Hauf, Elsner, & Aschersleben, 2004;Sommerville, Hildebrand, & Crane, 2008;Sommerville, Woodward, & Needham, 2005;Wang & Kohne, 2007;Wilcox, Woods, & Chapa, 2008).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%