2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Memory load affects object individuation in 18-month-old infants

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

6
46
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(52 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
6
46
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Meanwhile, studies like those reported here measure observers’ ability to remember that a given number of objects existed in a particular location, regardless of their features. Previous research shows that infants sometimes remember that an object was present, even when its features are forgotten (Kibbe & Feigenson, submitted; Kibbe & Leslie, 2011; Zosh & Feigenson, 2012), raising the possibility that heterogeneity may differently affect infants’ memory for objects’ existence and for their features. In addition, the influence of array heterogeneity at different retention intervals remains to be explored.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Meanwhile, studies like those reported here measure observers’ ability to remember that a given number of objects existed in a particular location, regardless of their features. Previous research shows that infants sometimes remember that an object was present, even when its features are forgotten (Kibbe & Feigenson, submitted; Kibbe & Leslie, 2011; Zosh & Feigenson, 2012), raising the possibility that heterogeneity may differently affect infants’ memory for objects’ existence and for their features. In addition, the influence of array heterogeneity at different retention intervals remains to be explored.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…But they showed little evidence of remembering the objects’ identities. For example, when a toy cat, shoe, and bus had been hidden but infants retrieved a car, duck, and brush (i.e., when all of the original objects changed their identities), infants failed to keep searching for the originally hidden objects (despite successfully using these identity changes to guide further searching when arrays of just one or two objects had originally been hidden; Zosh & Feigenson, 2012). In that study, infants presented with three-object arrays only succeeded at using object features to individuate when an object changed radically (from a solid plastic toy to a squishy, non-object blob).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Zosh and Feigenson (2012) arranged a switch so that 3 items hidden in the box (e.g., a cat, a shoe, and a bus) were not the same ones that were there to be retrieved from the box (e.g., a car, a duck, and a brush), yet these changes did not increase the time 18-month-olds spent looking in the box. The switches did make a difference when only 1 or 2 items were hidden in the box, so it appears that the feature information was lost (or at least, not usable in the same way) when too many items were added to working memory.…”
Section: Development Of Working Memory Capacity In Infancymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When the objects retrieved do not match the ones hidden it is possible that the infants even realize that something has changed, but are not committed to the specific features of the objects hidden when working memory is overwhelmed because their attention has shifted to using the limited available working memory to keep track of the number of items retrieved, with not enough capacity left to keep track of which objects have been retrieved. Some such account would help to explain why Zosh and Feigenson (2012) found that 18-month-old infants appeared to care about the switch given 1 or 2 hidden items but were unconcerned about the switch given 3 hidden items.…”
Section: Development Of Working Memory Capacity In Infancymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation