2014
DOI: 10.1037/a0037267
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The role of reward prediction in the control of attention.

Abstract: Previously rewarded stimuli involuntarily capture attention. The learning mechanisms underlying this value-driven attentional capture remain less understood. We tested whether theories of prediction-based associative reward learning explain the conditions under which reward feedback leads to value-based modulations of attentional priority. Across four experiments, we manipulated whether stimulus features served as unique predictors of reward outcomes. Participants received monetary rewards for correctly identi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
99
3
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

3
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 87 publications
(112 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
(100 reference statements)
9
99
3
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This is consistent with the reward learning that automatically guides attention being implicit in nature, relying on the co-occurrence of visual information and reward feedback rather than the establishment of strategic priorities that persist due to reinforcement, as has been suggested previously and elsewhere (e.g., Anderson et al, 2013; Anderson & Yantis, 2013; Buckner et al, this issue; Della Libera, Perlato, & Chelazzi, 2011; Sali, Anderson, & Yantis, in press; Tran et al, this issue). However, it should be noted that the evidence provided by the forced-choice question is only suggestive of implicit learning and cannot rule out awareness of the reward contingencies that either extinguished over the course of the test phase or was not sufficiently strong that participants were willing to endorse the correct contingency over random contingencies.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…This is consistent with the reward learning that automatically guides attention being implicit in nature, relying on the co-occurrence of visual information and reward feedback rather than the establishment of strategic priorities that persist due to reinforcement, as has been suggested previously and elsewhere (e.g., Anderson et al, 2013; Anderson & Yantis, 2013; Buckner et al, this issue; Della Libera, Perlato, & Chelazzi, 2011; Sali, Anderson, & Yantis, in press; Tran et al, this issue). However, it should be noted that the evidence provided by the forced-choice question is only suggestive of implicit learning and cannot rule out awareness of the reward contingencies that either extinguished over the course of the test phase or was not sufficiently strong that participants were willing to endorse the correct contingency over random contingencies.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…Note that this control experiment does not control for the possibility that the effects observed in Experiment 1 are due to motivational salience effects generated by the receipt of rewards. However, Sali, Anderson, and Yantis (2014) demonstrated in multiple experiments using a similar paradigm that motivational salience does not account for value-driven capture. In order to test these hypotheses, we performed the same analyses as in Experiment 1, the only difference being that instead of high-and low-reward conditions we have red and blue conditions.…”
Section: Cued Visual Search Taskmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The learned value of visual stimuli significantly affects attentional priority (Laurent, Hall, Anderson, & Yantis, 2015;Sali, Anderson, & Yantis, 2014). In basic eye-tracking experiments involving manipulation of object low level properties, participants quickly learned to search for objects of a property that would produce a valuable reward (Laurent et al, 2015;Sali et al, 2014).…”
Section: Search-related Expertisementioning
confidence: 99%