1996
DOI: 10.1016/s0166-4115(96)80057-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The role of attention in time estimation processes

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

21
260
0
6

Year Published

1998
1998
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 306 publications
(295 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
21
260
0
6
Order By: Relevance
“…The fact that this group effect was mediated by greater impulsivity of SDI can be explained by models of prospective time perception. Subjects estimate the duration of a given interval as longer when the focus of attention is on the passage of time as opposed to a condition where the same interval is filled with activities that distract an observer from attending to time (Wittmann and Lehnhoff, 2005;Zakay and Block, 1996). An overestimation of time intervals is a sign of boredom or emotional distress that draws attention away from meaningful thoughts and actions and directs it to the passage of time (Danckert and Allman, 2005;Twenge et al, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The fact that this group effect was mediated by greater impulsivity of SDI can be explained by models of prospective time perception. Subjects estimate the duration of a given interval as longer when the focus of attention is on the passage of time as opposed to a condition where the same interval is filled with activities that distract an observer from attending to time (Wittmann and Lehnhoff, 2005;Zakay and Block, 1996). An overestimation of time intervals is a sign of boredom or emotional distress that draws attention away from meaningful thoughts and actions and directs it to the passage of time (Danckert and Allman, 2005;Twenge et al, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Detailed analyses of disruptions in timing from interfering tasks such as those used in the present and other studies (e.g., Brown, 1997;Macar, 2002;Rammsayer & Ulrich, 2005) contribute to pinpoint the type of attentional resources involved when timing tasks are performed, a fundamental issue considering the central role of attention in most current influential models of timing (Gibbon, Church & Meck, 1984;Zakay & Block, 1996). Finally, from a practical perspective, given the use of timing tasks in measuring mental workload (e.g., Liu & Wickens, 1994), a more detailed definition of the processes involved in timing might also contribute to providing e a better index of the workload imposed by a variety of tasks (O'Donnell & Eggemeier, 1986 …”
mentioning
confidence: 84%
“…Indeed, there is evidence that acquisition, representation, and processing of temporal information in neural systems presumably differ from the processing of object or spatial stimulus features. For example, unlike stimulus dimensions like color and shape, temporal representations are acquired dynamically subserved by the use of effective encoding and monitoring strategies (Gilliland & Martin, 1940;Kileen & Weiss, 1987) and are thus specifically sensitive to attentional manipulations (Casini & Ivry, 1999;Mangels et al, 1998;Zakay & Block, 1996). The involvement of these strategic operations might have rendered the executive monitoring of duration information more sensitive to frontal lobe damage than the respective operations on spatial information.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Within the framework of internal clock models (Church, 1989;Gibbon, Church, & Meck, 1984;Treisman, Faulkner, Naish, & Brogan, 1990;Zakay & Block, 1996), which describe formal properties of the cognitive processes underlying duration processing, it is assumed that different components contribute each to a specific functional level of duration processing. Fundamental to this is the hypothesis that clock-counter mechanisms monitor the passage of subjective time (clock stage).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation