1926
DOI: 10.1080/01621459.1926.10502165
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Relative Merits of Circles and Bars for Representing Component Parts

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
47
0

Year Published

1991
1991
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 79 publications
(48 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
47
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, pie charts produced the smallest absolute error values in all three experiments. Despite the opinions of some critics (e.g., Cleveland, 1985;Macdonald-Ross, 1977;Tufte, 1983), the accumulated evidence suggests that the pie is an eective format for judgments of proportion and yields accuracies as good as or better than other graph types for that task (Eells, 1926;Hollands and Spence, 1992;Simkin and Hastie, 1987;Spence, 1990;Spence and Lewandowsky, 1991). It appears that the pie chart is the graph of choice for communicating part-towhole relationships, with divided bars and reference bars serving as reasonable alternatives.…”
Section: Implications For Designmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…However, pie charts produced the smallest absolute error values in all three experiments. Despite the opinions of some critics (e.g., Cleveland, 1985;Macdonald-Ross, 1977;Tufte, 1983), the accumulated evidence suggests that the pie is an eective format for judgments of proportion and yields accuracies as good as or better than other graph types for that task (Eells, 1926;Hollands and Spence, 1992;Simkin and Hastie, 1987;Spence, 1990;Spence and Lewandowsky, 1991). It appears that the pie chart is the graph of choice for communicating part-towhole relationships, with divided bars and reference bars serving as reasonable alternatives.…”
Section: Implications For Designmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…Walter Eells (1926) apparently had shown the superiority of the pie chart over the divided horizontal bar chart. In a simple but well-executed experiment he had subjects estimate the percentage associated with a single component (a slice of the pie or an individual bar) and found that the magnitude estimation was performed more accurately and more quickly when the data were in pie chart form.…”
Section: Early Experiments On Statistical Graphsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition to the fact that the shape of the sub-area is idiosyncratic, subjects can use perceptual anchor points that are otherwise unavailable: with the pie chart it is easy to compare the size of a component with imaginary quarters, or halves, as well as the whole, before making either absolute or relative judgements. A crude linear scale is thus available to the observer, and subjects report making use of this scale (Eells, 1926;Simkin and Hastie, 1987;Spence, 1989). Indeed, Spence (1989) found that the estimated exponent for the judged size of pie chart segments is about one.…”
Section: The Psychophysics Of Magnitude Estimationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Criticisms like this provoked the first psychological experiments on graphs, where subjects were required to estimate quantities represented in graphic form. Eells (1926) showed that subjects could estimate the size of a proportion more quickly and accurately when the data were in pie chart rather than bar chart form. His advocacy quickly produced critics who conducted their own experiments (Croxton, 1927;Croxton & Stein, 1932;Croxton & Stryker, 1927;von Huhn, 1927), but these early empirical studies were of widely varying quality, were generally inconclusive, and did not convincingly reverse Eells's findings.…”
Section: Empirical Evaluation Of the Pie Chart And Other Formsmentioning
confidence: 99%