2002
DOI: 10.1159/000049948
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Relative Importance of Size of Food and Interfood Distance in Eliciting Aggression in Captive Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta)

Abstract: We conducted an experiment on a group of captive rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) in which we manipulated both food size and interfood distance independently to examine which factor was more important in causing aggressive competition. For each of 254 trials, the monkeys were offered simultaneously two apple pieces ranging in size from 1 to 40 g at interfood distances ranging from 1 to 5 m. In contrast to other studies, food size and interfood distance were not conflated in this study. Multiple regression anal… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

1
32
0

Year Published

2002
2002
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 83 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
1
32
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In an experimental study, Schaub (1995) found that subordinate longtailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) were less likely to obtain foods when the dominant animals were closer to them, but he could only test them at distances of 30 cm and 100 cm. A recent study of captive rhesus macaques (Mathy & Isbell, 2001) suggests that 100 cm is well within the zone of monopolizabilit y of higher-ranking animals when there is no physical barrier between the animals, as was the case in Schaub's experiment. In Mathy and Isbell's study, group-living individual s were provided with two apple pieces simultaneously.…”
Section: Food Distributio N and Dominance Hierarchiesmentioning
confidence: 88%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In an experimental study, Schaub (1995) found that subordinate longtailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) were less likely to obtain foods when the dominant animals were closer to them, but he could only test them at distances of 30 cm and 100 cm. A recent study of captive rhesus macaques (Mathy & Isbell, 2001) suggests that 100 cm is well within the zone of monopolizabilit y of higher-ranking animals when there is no physical barrier between the animals, as was the case in Schaub's experiment. In Mathy and Isbell's study, group-living individual s were provided with two apple pieces simultaneously.…”
Section: Food Distributio N and Dominance Hierarchiesmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…The models make arguments about patch sizes across a wide range of spatial scales, but it is not clear what scale is appropriate for different kinds of competition. Recognizing this, Isbell and colleagues have attempted to examine more speci cally the operational aspects of food distributio n as they affect competition (Isbell & Pruetz, 1998;Isbell et al, 1998;Pruetz & Isbell, 2000;Mathy & Isbell, 2001). It appears that the patchiness of foods per se is not what matters.…”
Section: Food Distributio N and Dominance Hierarchiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore, it is predicted that higher-quality foods [Koenig et al, 1998;Range & Nöe, 2002] or food items in clumped distribution [Mathy & Isbell, 2001] are more likely to be monopolized. Defending higher-quality foods is rewarded by gaining a higher intake of energy and nutrition.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The interaction between the spatial and the temporal availability of food resources will determine the possibility of monopolizing food patches [Goss-Custard et al, 1984;Grenier et al, 1999;Weir and Grant, 2004]. For instance, when food occurs in discrete patches, spatial clumping and temporal dispersion of patches may promote resource monopolization, whereas spatial dispersal and high abundance of food reduces the defensibility of food patches [Goldberg et al, 2001;Mathy and Isbell, 2001]. When food monopolization occurs, within-group feeding competition may emerge, either directly, through the expression of ritualized or aggressive behaviours, or indirectly, through dominance relationships [Goss-Custard et al, 1984;Grenier et al, 1999;Mathy and Isbell, 2001;Weir and Grant, 2004].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, when food occurs in discrete patches, spatial clumping and temporal dispersion of patches may promote resource monopolization, whereas spatial dispersal and high abundance of food reduces the defensibility of food patches [Goldberg et al, 2001;Mathy and Isbell, 2001]. When food monopolization occurs, within-group feeding competition may emerge, either directly, through the expression of ritualized or aggressive behaviours, or indirectly, through dominance relationships [Goss-Custard et al, 1984;Grenier et al, 1999;Mathy and Isbell, 2001;Weir and Grant, 2004]. In addition to its influence on the establishment and maintenance of social relationships among group members, feeding competition represents a stressor to many animal species, as attested by changes in the secretion of glucocorticoids (e.g.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%