1997
DOI: 10.3758/bf03214205
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The perception of shape and curvedness from binocular stereopsis and structure from motion

Abstract: The integration of binocular stereopsis and kinetic depth was measured for two distinct aspects of 3-Dstructure: (1) shape index, which is a measure of scale-independent structure, and (2) curvedness, which is a measure of scale-dependent structure. We found that motion contributes significantly more to judged shape index than it does to judged curvedness, and stereo contributes significantly more to judged curvedness than it does to judged shape index. This suggests that the differences in the relative contri… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
13
0

Year Published

2000
2000
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
0
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, they are not inconsisten t with the idea that the tasks accessed separate representations using, for example, separate representations of slant and of absolute depth (c.f. Tittle and Perotti, 1997), or independent representations supporting perception and the control of action (Milner and Goodale, 1995). However, if multiple representations are involved, then they are clearly equally affected by the stereoscopic anisotropy.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…However, they are not inconsisten t with the idea that the tasks accessed separate representations using, for example, separate representations of slant and of absolute depth (c.f. Tittle and Perotti, 1997), or independent representations supporting perception and the control of action (Milner and Goodale, 1995). However, if multiple representations are involved, then they are clearly equally affected by the stereoscopic anisotropy.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Only when determining the absolute size and shape of an object is a full speci cation of metric Euclidean structure required. Tittle and Perotti (1997) similarly proposed that different tasks make use of different depth representations but, rather than the hierarchical framework discussed by Glennerster et al (1996), suggest that multiple independent representations of depth structure are produced. Therefore, in their model a representation which describes the three-dimensiona l positions of points may exist side-by-side with a representation of higher order properties such as surface slant, or curvature.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 95%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…A quantity that has been studied in the structure-frommotion literature is curvature (Koenderink, 1990;Perotti et al, 1998;Tittle & Perotti, 1997). There, it has been used to describe the structure of perceived surfaces.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Humans are adept at detecting weak coherent motion amidst a background of incoherent motion (Raymond, 1994;van de Grind, Koenderink, van Doorn, Milders, & Voerman, 1993;Williams & Sekuler, 1984; but see Barlow & Tripathy, 1997), are very accurate at judging the direction in which objects are moving (Gros, Blake, & Hiris, 1998;De Bruyn & Orban, 1988;Pasternak & Merigan, 1984;Ball & Sekuler, 1982), and, under optimal conditions, are keenly sensitive to slight differences in the speed at which objects are moving (Chen, Bedell, & Frishman, 1998;McKee & Welch, 1989;De Bruyn & Orban, 1988;Orban, De Wolf, & Maes, 1984). In addition, motion provides a potent source of information for specifying the 3-D shapes of objects (Tittle & Perotti, 1998;Sperling, Landy, Dosher, & Perkins, 1989;Lappin & Fuqua, 1983;Rogers & Graham, 1979;Wallach & O'Connell, 1953).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%