The platform will undergo maintenance on Sep 14 at about 7:45 AM EST and will be unavailable for approximately 2 hours.
1991
DOI: 10.1007/bf00230973
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The neural basis of the behaviorally relevant N1 component of the somatosensory-evoked potential in SI cortex of awake monkeys: evidence that backward cortical projections signal conscious touch sensation

Abstract: Studies of touch intensity discrimination in monkeys have identified a component of the somatosensory-evoked cortical potential, N1, generated within 50 ms of the stimulus, that predicts their behavioral performance. This study employed multiple-electrode arrays with relatively high spatial resolution (0.1 or 0.2 mm spacing) to record laminar profiles of somatosensory-evoked potentials (SEPs), multiple unit activity (MUA) and current source-densities (CSDs) at several sites across the postcentral gyri of two a… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

10
94
0

Year Published

1998
1998
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 132 publications
(104 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
10
94
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Importantly, there is substantial evidence to suggest that top-down processes not only play an important role in sensory processing in general but also are crucial for conscious perception, in particular [75][76][77][78][79][80][81][82] (see also the review articles by Pollen, 83 Lamme and Roelfsema, 84 Bullier, 85 Hochstein and Ahissar, 86 and Meyer 87 ). For example, when subjects perceive apparent motion (as is the case when two dots in different locations are seen in rapid alteration, creating the impression that a single dot is moving from one location to the other), there is V1 activity along the apparent motion trace (where there is no actual visual stimulus), and this activity is induced by top-down signals.…”
Section: Top-down Signals and Conscious Perception: What We Already Knowmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Importantly, there is substantial evidence to suggest that top-down processes not only play an important role in sensory processing in general but also are crucial for conscious perception, in particular [75][76][77][78][79][80][81][82] (see also the review articles by Pollen, 83 Lamme and Roelfsema, 84 Bullier, 85 Hochstein and Ahissar, 86 and Meyer 87 ). For example, when subjects perceive apparent motion (as is the case when two dots in different locations are seen in rapid alteration, creating the impression that a single dot is moving from one location to the other), there is V1 activity along the apparent motion trace (where there is no actual visual stimulus), and this activity is induced by top-down signals.…”
Section: Top-down Signals and Conscious Perception: What We Already Knowmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…77 Similar findings exist for the somatosensory modality: the subjective intensity of tactile stimuli is reflected in top-down signals that reach layer 1 of the primary somatosensory cortex from higher-level areas rather than in the thalamocortical signals that initially arrive in layer 4. 75,76 As an additional observation, which applies equally to the visual, auditory, and somatosensory modalities, there is an interdependency of the latency of sensory cortex responses and their correlation with conscious experience: although early activity in the sensory areas appears to be strictly stimulus bound, later activity, which reflects top-down signals from higher-order cortices, is correlated more closely with the subject's conscious percept. 24,75,76,79,89,90 Thus, several lines of evidence from neuroanatomy, neurophysiology, and functional neuroimaging suggest that top-down signaling fulfills an indispensable function in conscious perception.…”
Section: Top-down Signals and Conscious Perception: What We Already Knowmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The origin of P1 from SI-specific thalamocortical inputs is well established (Mitzdorf, 1985). N1 is associated with the slow, long latency sink in layers I/II (Agmon and Connors, 1991;Kulics and Cauller, 1986) and is generated by excitatory cortical events (Cauller and Kulics, 1991). N1 is reduced during deep anesthesia (Arezzo et al, 1981) and slow-wave sleep (Cauller and Kulics, 1988).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One leading hypothesis is that during awake sensory processing, thalamo-cortical interactions involving deep-layer pyramidal neurons result in non-specific thalamic nuclei projecting feedback input to the tuft dendrites of L5 pyramidal neurons (4). It is also hypothesized that higher cortical areas (more active during conscious processing) project to the same tuft dendrites in L1 in primary sensory areas (5,6). Both these hypotheses predict specific input to pyramidal cell dendrites during the awake state.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%