2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The long and the short of it: On the nature and origin of functional overlap between representations of space and time

Abstract: When we describe time, we often use the language of space (The movie was long; The deadline is approaching). Experiments 1–3 asked whether—as patterns in language suggest—a structural similarity between representations of spatial length and temporal duration is easier to access than one between length and other dimensions of experience, such as loudness. Adult participants were shown pairings of lines of different length with tones of different duration (Experiment 1) or tones of different loudness (Experiment… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
126
2
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 190 publications
(141 citation statements)
references
References 93 publications
9
126
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…As such, more external (and embodied) word meanings are predicted to be well understood and richly structured, and to provide a source of mappings for ideas that are more difficult to understand. Alternatively, our results may not reveal that abstract concepts are understood in terms of external and embodied concepts, per se, but rather, that external and embodied word meanings are easier to achieve shared reference for and to learn (Srinivasan & Carey, 2010;Thibodeau & Durgin, 2008) because of their salience and physical availability in experience. It is also possible that external meanings tend to be more frequent, and may thus be easier to retrieve for speakers who wish to communicate new ideas (Hanley et al, 2013;cf.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 66%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…As such, more external (and embodied) word meanings are predicted to be well understood and richly structured, and to provide a source of mappings for ideas that are more difficult to understand. Alternatively, our results may not reveal that abstract concepts are understood in terms of external and embodied concepts, per se, but rather, that external and embodied word meanings are easier to achieve shared reference for and to learn (Srinivasan & Carey, 2010;Thibodeau & Durgin, 2008) because of their salience and physical availability in experience. It is also possible that external meanings tend to be more frequent, and may thus be easier to retrieve for speakers who wish to communicate new ideas (Hanley et al, 2013;cf.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 66%
“…Three of the variables we considered as predictors are suggested by Conceptual Metaphor Theory (Lakoff & Johnson, 1980;Reddy, 1979;Sweetser, 1991) and supported by other perspectives suggesting that certain types of meanings should more commonly serve as sources of metaphorical extensions (e.g., Carey, 2010, andThibodeau &Durgin, 2008, as discussed above; see also Traugott, 1990). …”
Section: Candidate Variablesmentioning
confidence: 84%
“…One hallmark study by Boroditsky and Ramscar (2002) found that time can be grounded in the concrete experience of space. However, Srinivasan and Carey (2010) found that comparable time-space effects are already detected in preverbal infants. Thus, such abstract representations might be explainable through either embodied processes, or a process of scaffolding (see also IJzerman and Koole, 2011;IJzerman et al, 2015b).…”
Section: Individual Differences: the Need For Prospectionmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…In addition, 9-mo-olds can readily learn pairs of positively (but not negatively) correlated line lengths and tone durations (60), suggesting that infants at least can represent an abstract "more-than" and "less-than" representation that applies to both dimensions. However, 9-mo-old infants do not show equal sensitivity to monotonic pairings between the dimensions of loudness and space as they do for pairing of space and time (60). Those findings suggest that there may be an asymmetry between magnitudes in their intrinsic ordinal associations.…”
Section: For Review)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One recent study showed that 9-mo-olds were equally likely to transfer an arbitrary, experimentally learned magnitude-to-texture association from one dimension (e.g., number) to another dimension (size or duration) (59). In addition, 9-mo-olds can readily learn pairs of positively (but not negatively) correlated line lengths and tone durations (60), suggesting that infants at least can represent an abstract "more-than" and "less-than" representation that applies to both dimensions. However, 9-mo-old infants do not show equal sensitivity to monotonic pairings between the dimensions of loudness and space as they do for pairing of space and time (60).…”
Section: For Review)mentioning
confidence: 99%