2017
DOI: 10.3917/rfla.222.0089
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The interactional achievement of tellability: a study of story-openings

Abstract: Cet article s’intéresse au récit dans la conversation ordinaire en français. Si le récit est un objet d’étude privilégié des sciences humaines et sociales, notre attention se porte ici sur la contribution de l’Analyse Conversationnelle. Les travaux en AC ont mis en évidence la nature interactionnelle des récits, qui sont accomplis à travers les ajustements mutuels des participants tour-après-tour. Après un aperçu des principaux résultats de cette approche, nous présentons une étude sur les ouvertures de récit.… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…While some of these utterances are part of story-prefaces that prepare the grounds for an incipient negative telling (cf. Sacks, 1974 ; Jefferson, 1978 ; Berger, 2017 ; see Excerpt 3 ), not all utterances precede stories (see Excerpt 2 , 4 ). Considering the contingent nature of complaints, which require coparticipant collaboration to come about ( Traverso, 2009 ; Ruusuvuori et al, 2019 ), embodied displays of stance, whether produced as part of a verbal-embodied package or self-standing (for the latter, see Skogmyr Marian, 2020 , 2021 ), seem to be a way for future complainants to ‘test the waters’ of their negatively valenced course of action before launching the complaint fully.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While some of these utterances are part of story-prefaces that prepare the grounds for an incipient negative telling (cf. Sacks, 1974 ; Jefferson, 1978 ; Berger, 2017 ; see Excerpt 3 ), not all utterances precede stories (see Excerpt 2 , 4 ). Considering the contingent nature of complaints, which require coparticipant collaboration to come about ( Traverso, 2009 ; Ruusuvuori et al, 2019 ), embodied displays of stance, whether produced as part of a verbal-embodied package or self-standing (for the latter, see Skogmyr Marian, 2020 , 2021 ), seem to be a way for future complainants to ‘test the waters’ of their negatively valenced course of action before launching the complaint fully.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The example is similar to the one about the champagne in that the self-generalization is presenting something that might be contrary to the perceived norms of society, and this per se seems to be a motivating factor for producing them; they index the tellability of the story (see Berger, 2017) by anchoring it through a description of the ‘characteristics’ of the person launching it. Also, as in example 1, the self-generalization can be seen to be orienting to a possible complainable, in this case in particular the cultural norm that one ought to be a good mother; the self-generalization can thus be seen to be commenting on the ‘wrongness’ of the account to come (see Drew, 1998: 295, 302), displaying an orientation to the possibility that one may be considered to have acted improperly.…”
Section: Self-descriptions In Finnish Conversations: Accounting For One’s Behaviormentioning
confidence: 90%
“…Besides the function of projecting incipient storytelling and making the story coherent with the on-going talk, story-openings may also be used to justify the ‘tellability’ (i.e. why it is worth telling) (Berger, 2017; Sacks, 1974), or establish the ‘storyworthiness’ (i.e. why it is worth listening) (Beach, 2000).…”
Section: Ca Research On Storytelling and Story-openingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As a consequence, our current understanding of story-opening relies on the existing research on ordinary storytelling, which has uncovered several typical interactional functions. These include turn-taking management, that is, projecting an incipient story which requires a multi-unit turn (Sacks, 1974); sequence organization, that is, making the story cohere with the on-going talk (Dingemanse et al, 2017; Jefferson, 1978); and recipient management, that is, justifying the tellability of the story, foreshadowing the type of story, and giving hints to recipients regarding how to interpret and react to the story (Berger, 2017; Dingemanse et al, 2017; Jefferson, 1978; Sacks, 1974).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%