2018
DOI: 10.1101/363606
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The influence of task outcome on implicit motor learning

Abstract: 9Recent studies have demonstrated that task success signals can modulate learning during 10 sensorimotor adaptation tasks, primarily through engaging explicit processes. Here we examine the 11 influence of task outcome on implicit adaptation, using a reaching task in which adaptation is induced 12 by feedback that is not contingent on actual performance. We imposed an invariant perturbation 13 (rotation) on the feedback cursor while varying the target size. In this way, the cursor either hit or 14 missed the t… Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

9
96
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(106 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
(22 reference statements)
9
96
1
Order By: Relevance
“…These results are consistent with the range of implicit adaptation previously found in numerous studies, including those using standard visuomotor rotation tasks without re-aiming reports (Taylor and Ivry, 2014), perturbations introduced in a pseudorandom walk (Stark-Inbar, 2017), and taskirrelevant-visual-error clamp Kim et al 2018;Kim et al 2019). Despite these individual differences, participants appeared to be remarkably consistent in maintaining their personal relative contribution of explicit and implicit learning within and across days.…”
Section: Analysis Of Within-and Between-subject Variancesupporting
confidence: 88%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These results are consistent with the range of implicit adaptation previously found in numerous studies, including those using standard visuomotor rotation tasks without re-aiming reports (Taylor and Ivry, 2014), perturbations introduced in a pseudorandom walk (Stark-Inbar, 2017), and taskirrelevant-visual-error clamp Kim et al 2018;Kim et al 2019). Despite these individual differences, participants appeared to be remarkably consistent in maintaining their personal relative contribution of explicit and implicit learning within and across days.…”
Section: Analysis Of Within-and Between-subject Variancesupporting
confidence: 88%
“…This result may have been expected given recent evidence that implicit adaptation falls well short of full learning (Morehead and Smith, 2017;Morehead, Taylor, Parvin, and Ivry, 2017;Kim et al, 2018;Kim, Parvin, and Ivry, 2019). We hypothesized that this failure of implicit adaptation was due to a lack of sufficient opportunity for consolidation, an idea that failed to be supported by our five-day experiments.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 55%
“…The first group adapted fully to the rotation and had eye movements consistent with performance in exclusion trials (Match-High group); the second group also adapted fully but had less explicit eye than would be expected from exclusion trials (No-Match group); the third group only adapted partially and had eye movements consistent with lack of explicit adaptation in the exclusion trials (Match-Low group). The learning curves of this last group were similar to those reported in paradigms where subjects had only implicit adaptation (Morehead et al, 2017;Kim et al, 2018Kim et al, , 2019. Smith et al (2006) proposed a two-state model of motor adaptation that is still the most widely used model in the field.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 73%
“…McDougle et al propose that explicit re-aiming may have different components that are computed and cached (McDougle and Taylor 2019). Others propose that implicit adaptation has labile and stable components (Miyamoto, Wang et al 2014, Kim, Parvin et al 2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%