1999
DOI: 10.1006/brln.1999.2106
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Influence of Morphological Regularities on the Dynamics of a Connectionist Network

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
82
3

Year Published

1999
1999
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 81 publications
(93 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
7
82
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Until these kinds of methodological questions are resolved, it remains difficult to use neuroimaging data to adjudicate between competing accounts of the functional and neural correlates of morphological complexity including distributed connectionist accounts in which decomposition arises from overlapping representations of morphologically related forms and where no specific neural mechanisms is required to handle this process (e.g. Davis, van Casteren, & Marslen-Wilson, 2003;Plaut & Gonnerman, 2000;Rueckl & Raveh, 1999).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Until these kinds of methodological questions are resolved, it remains difficult to use neuroimaging data to adjudicate between competing accounts of the functional and neural correlates of morphological complexity including distributed connectionist accounts in which decomposition arises from overlapping representations of morphologically related forms and where no specific neural mechanisms is required to handle this process (e.g. Davis, van Casteren, & Marslen-Wilson, 2003;Plaut & Gonnerman, 2000;Rueckl & Raveh, 1999).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A third possible account can be suggested in keeping with connectionist distributed models of morphology which posit that morphological priming effects emerge as a result of a synergistic interaction between form and meaning (Plaut & Gonnerman, 2000;Rueckl et al, 1997;Rueckl & Raveh, 1999). Two aspects of the etymon as an organizing unit seem problematic for this account.…”
Section: Non-concatenative Morphological Processing In a Wider Contextmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Irrespective of how morphological decomposition is conceptualized, there has been relative agreement that morphologically complex words share representations with their stems only in cases in which there is a semantic relationship between them (MarslenWilson et al, 1994; see also Davis et al, 2003;Giraudo & Grainger, 2000;Plaut & Gonnerman, 2000;Rueckl & Raveh, 1999;Rueckl et al, 1997). For example, all these theories of morphological processing would take the view that representations of darkness and dark overlap to a much greater degree than the representations of witness and wit (which are etymologically related, but are no longer semantically related) or corner and corn (which are not semantically or, despite appearances, etymologically related).…”
Section: Morphological Analysis In the Absence Of Semantics?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…''Classical'' theories (e.g., Giraudo & Grainger, 2000;Marslen-Wilson, Tyler, Waksler, & Older, 1994) postulate a level at which morphemic units are represented explicitly and at which morphologically complex words are segmented into their constituents. Distributedconnectionist theories (e.g., Davis, van Casteren, & Marslen-Wilson, 2003;Plaut & Gonnerman, 2000;Rueckl & Raveh, 1999;Rueckl, Mikolinsky, Raveh, Miner, & Mars, 1997), by contrast, do not postulate an explicit level of morphological representation, but instead develop highly similar representations for morphologically complex words and their stems in the hidden units mediating orthographic and semantic representations (see Rastle et al, 2000, for a discussion). Morphological structure emerges in these distributed-connectionist models as a consequence of the consistency that morphologically complex words bring to the mapping between orthography and meaning.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%