2020
DOI: 10.1038/s41467-020-16196-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The impact of learning on perceptual decisions and its implication for speed-accuracy tradeoffs

Abstract: In standard models of perceptual decision-making, noisy sensory evidence is considered to be the primary source of choice errors and the accumulation of evidence needed to overcome this noise gives rise to speed-accuracy tradeoffs. Here, we investigated how the history of recent choices and their outcomes interact with these processes using a combination of theory and experiment. We found that the speed and accuracy of performance of rats on olfactory decision tasks could be best explained by a Bayesian model … Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
36
0
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(43 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
1
36
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…It would be interesting to apply a modified version of the DT model to perceptual decision-making tasks to attempt to capture such lapse behavior. Furthermore, in perceptual decision-making tasks that expose animals to a static environment, models that assume a dynamic environment are better at capturing the animals’ choices [ 48 ]. However, when an environment is not static and task-relevant stimuli exhibit autocorrelations or bias, choice history effects carry an important adaptive function [ 49 51 ].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It would be interesting to apply a modified version of the DT model to perceptual decision-making tasks to attempt to capture such lapse behavior. Furthermore, in perceptual decision-making tasks that expose animals to a static environment, models that assume a dynamic environment are better at capturing the animals’ choices [ 48 ]. However, when an environment is not static and task-relevant stimuli exhibit autocorrelations or bias, choice history effects carry an important adaptive function [ 49 51 ].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…That discrete states underpin mouse choice behavior may also call for new normative models to explain why mice may develop these states to begin with [1]. The existence of disengaged and engaged states could reflect explore-exploit behavior [15, 16, 50], optimal learning (e.g., [4, 23, 26, 47]), or could simply indicate incomplete learning of the task. Another promising direction will be to replace the model’s fixed transition matrix with a generalized linear model allowing external covariates to modulate the probability of state changes [12]).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The MF correction exploits the idea that the effect of slow drifts should be similar for at least a small stretch of trials (n>=2) and any influence of future rewards on past choices is acausal, likely due to slow drifts and needs to be removed. (Mendonça et al 2020) proposed the variant of the correction for choice-outcome biases and (Lak, Hueske, et al 2020) for stimulus history biases. We describe and test the efficacy of both these variants here.…”
Section: Correction In Actionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This dependence is thought to arise from systematic updating of decision variables from trial to trial. These updates may reflect ongoing learning (Dayan and Daw 2008), for instance an agent learning to perform a perceptual categorization task might update its beliefs about the prior probabilities of the different categories (Yu and Cohen 2008;Zhang, Huang, and Yu 2014), the category boundary separating them (Drugowitsch et al 2019;Mendonça et al 2020), or the values of the available actions (Lak, Okun, et al 2020;Lak, Hueske, et al 2020;Pisupati et al 2021). Alternatively, systematic updates may reflect heuristic strategies adopted by decision-makers due to resource constraints or mismatched objectives (Gigerenzer and Gaissmaier 2011;Abrahamyan et al 2016;Gardner 2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation