1996
DOI: 10.1006/obhd.1996.0074
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Hard–Easy Effect in Subjective Probability Calibration

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

5
69
0

Year Published

1997
1997
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 136 publications
(75 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
5
69
0
Order By: Relevance
“…quality of the evidence (for a similar argument see Ferrell & McGoey, 1980;Suantak et al, 1996). Overall accuracy.…”
Section: Accuracy Of Confidence Ratingsmentioning
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…quality of the evidence (for a similar argument see Ferrell & McGoey, 1980;Suantak et al, 1996). Overall accuracy.…”
Section: Accuracy Of Confidence Ratingsmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…This assumption is true for signal detection models of confidence (e.g., Budescu, Wallsten, & Au, 1997;Macmillan & Creelman, 2005;Suantak, Bolger, & Ferrell, 1996) as well as a majority of sequential sampling models of confidence (e.g., Heath, 1984;Link, 2003;Merkle & Van Zandt, 2006;Moreno-Bote, in press;Van Zandt, 2000b;Vickers, 1979Vickers, , 2001. We take a different course.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…That is, when confidence is high, it's generally too high, and when it's low, it's generally too low-the typical pattern of miscalibration. Similarly, when accuracy is low for a set of questions it's likely to be lower than the available information would lead one to expect, and when accuracy is high it's likely to be higher than one would expect (Dawes & Mulford, 1996;Klayman et al, 1999;Suantak, Bolger, & Ferrell, 1996). This leads to overconfidence for "hard" question sets, and occasionally even underconfidence for "easy" question sets -the hardeasy effect.…”
Section: Overconfidence In Interval Estimatesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Confidence has been studied in humans using a variety of techniques (Balakrishnan 1999;García-Pérez andAlcalá-Quintana 2011, 2012;Garcia-Perez and Peli 2014;Hsu and Doble 2015;Okamoto 2012;Sawides et al 2013) including probability judgments (Baranski and Petrusic 1994;Björkman 1994;Ferrell 1995;Ferrell and McGoey 1980;Juslin et al 1998;Keren 1991;Lichtenstein et al 1982;Stankov 1998;Stankov et al 2012;Suantak et al 1996). In fact, as noted in a recent review (Grimaldi et al 2015), confidence probability judgments (i.e., confidence ratings provided using a nearly continuous scale between 0 and 100% or 50 and 100%) provide the most common assessment of confidence.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One common use of confidence recordings is in "confidence calibration" studies where confidence is compared with actual performance, where a data set may be classified as "wellcalibrated" or classified as indicative of "overconfidence" or "underconfidence" (Baranski and Petrusic 1994;Björkman 1994;Ferrell and McGoey 1980;Juslin et al 1998;Keren 1991;Lichtenstein et al 1982;Stankov 1998;Suantak et al 1996). Specifically, imagine that a subject reported 90% confidence that a given motion was rightward for 10 separate trials at a given stimulus level.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%