2004
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.299
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Overconfidence in Interval Estimates.

Abstract: Judges were asked to make numerical estimates (e.g., “In what year was the first flight of a hot air balloon?”). Judges provided high and low estimates such that they were X% sure that the correct answer lay between them. They exhibited substantial overconfidence: The correct answer fell inside their intervals much less than X% of the time. This contrasts with choices between 2 possible answers to a question, which showed much less overconfidence. The authors show that overconfidence in interval estimates can … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

25
359
4
5

Year Published

2004
2004
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 347 publications
(393 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
(102 reference statements)
25
359
4
5
Order By: Relevance
“…The consistent finding from lab evidence is that men are more risk-prone than women (see surveys by Grossman, 2008, andGneezy, 2009). While some attribute these differences to the emotional reaction to uncertain situations (Lowenstein et al, 2001), others report that they are related to confidence (Soll and Klayman, 2004). Similar conclusions have been drawn from (scant) survey and field evidence (Dohmen et al, 2011;Dwyer et al, 2011).…”
Section: Individual Preferencessupporting
confidence: 71%
“…The consistent finding from lab evidence is that men are more risk-prone than women (see surveys by Grossman, 2008, andGneezy, 2009). While some attribute these differences to the emotional reaction to uncertain situations (Lowenstein et al, 2001), others report that they are related to confidence (Soll and Klayman, 2004). Similar conclusions have been drawn from (scant) survey and field evidence (Dohmen et al, 2011;Dwyer et al, 2011).…”
Section: Individual Preferencessupporting
confidence: 71%
“…and then have participants estimate 90% confidence intervals around their answers. Results show that these confidence intervals are too narrow, suggesting that people are too sure they know the correct answer; 90% confidence intervals contain the correct answer less than 50% of the time (Alpert & Raiffa, 1969Klayman, Soll, Gonzalez-Vallejo, & Barlas, 1999;Soll & Klayman, 2004).…”
Section: Trouble With Overconfidencementioning
confidence: 96%
“…The usual finding is that for all questions assigned a given probability the proportion of correct answers is lower than the assigned probability [Lichtenstein et al, 1982]. The result that people form probability distributions over uncertain quantities that are too tight seems to be robust especially when people judge difficult items [see Klayman et al, 1999 or Soll andKlayman, 2004].…”
Section: Miscalibrationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…See the CEPR version of this paper for details (Glaser and Weber [2003]). 22 See Section 3.1.1 and, for example, Cesarini et al [2006], Klayman et al [1999], Biais et al [2005], Soll and Klayman [2004]. 23 Seven investors answered one question, three investors answered two questions, four investors answered three questions, and nine investors answered four questions.…”
Section: Miscalibration (Misc)mentioning
confidence: 99%