1974
DOI: 10.1016/0300-9629(74)90709-9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effects of temperature, anoxia and sensory stimulation on the heart rate of unrestrained crabs

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
46
0

Year Published

1977
1977
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 81 publications
(46 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
0
46
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Cumberlidge & Uglow 1977), whereas gradual changes in both amplitude and frequency seem to be a normal feature for Saduria. The decapod heart reacts immediately to external stimuli (Florey & Kriebel 1974) but Saduria hearts seem to continue their cyclical beating regardless of external variables. The cardiac arrests that occur in normoxia and moderate hypoxia are thus normal events in the beat cycle: slower and slower until total stop, then gradually starting again.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Cumberlidge & Uglow 1977), whereas gradual changes in both amplitude and frequency seem to be a normal feature for Saduria. The decapod heart reacts immediately to external stimuli (Florey & Kriebel 1974) but Saduria hearts seem to continue their cyclical beating regardless of external variables. The cardiac arrests that occur in normoxia and moderate hypoxia are thus normal events in the beat cycle: slower and slower until total stop, then gradually starting again.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Larimer and Tindel (1966) observed CA when crayfish perceived the presence of food (chemical sensing or contact). These authors did not describe heartrate fluctuations in animal-animal interactions, but some evidence has been found that the visual detection of movement of a conspecific animal produces CA (Cuadras, 1979;Florey & Kriebel, 1974). Stimulation to produce CA (touching eyes, antennae, mouthparts, legs, or carapace) was ineffective if the bottom of the tank was covered with a layer of fine gravel that would allow the crabs to burrow into it (Florey & Kriebel, 1974), but visual detection of movement was effective in stopping the heart in sandy-bottom tanks (Cuadras, 1979(Cuadras, , 1980.…”
Section: Sensory Modalities Involved In Cardiac Inhibitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A moving target elicited CA in Cancer (Wilkens et al, 1974) and in Dardanus (Cuadras, 1980), in the latter case associated with a startle response. When the crabs were approached by observers, bradycardia (Depledge, 1978) or CA (Florey & Kriebel, 1974), as well as startle (Cuadras, 1979), occurred. When behavior was also recorded, CA appeared to be a physiological component of the startle response (Cuadras, 1980, Note 1), regardless of the ensuing pattern of behavior and specific cardiac activity that usually followed.…”
Section: Sensory Modalities Involved In Cardiac Inhibitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, the responses to several types of stimuli (tactile and chemical cues) were examined for their effects on the heart and ventilatory rate of crayfish, finding a reflex inhibition in the majority of them (Larimer, 1964). More specifically, bradycardia or reversible heart arrests have been reported in crabs, lobsters and crayfish to a variety of optical and tactile stimuli (Cuadras, 1980;Cumberlidge and Uglow, 1977;Florey and Kriebel, 1974;Grober, 1990a;Grober, 1990b;Larimer and Tindel, 1966;McMahon and Wilkens, 1972;Mislin, 1966;Shuranova and Burmistrov, 2002;Uglow, 1973;Wilkens et al, 1974). Furthermore, another set of results also gathered in crustacea have shown that even though no observable behavioral responses were elicited, heart rate was measurably affected by small disturbances in the environment or by social interaction (Li et al, 2000;Listerman et al, 2000;Schapker et al, 2002).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%