1999
DOI: 10.3758/bf03199426
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The dolphin’s grammatical competency: Comments on Kako (1999)

Abstract: Kako (1999)reviews the evidence for syntactic competencies in several animal species exposed to artificial language systems, emphasizing the importance of core syntactic properties such as argument structure and closed-classitems. Wepresent evidence from our dolphin studies for the acquisition ofthe closed-class functionality of demonstratives, prepositions, conjunctions, and locatives. Sensitivityto argument structure is also evidenced by wholly untrained and consistent interpretations of the dolphin to probe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
19
0

Year Published

1999
1999
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
0
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We are therefore inclined to believe that it is arbitrary whether these tasks are called artificial grammar tasks or artificial orthographic regularity tasks. In this sense, the present research is quite different from research on language or communication in dolphins, parrots, chimpanzees, and other nonhuman animals, as well as in humans (Calvin & Bickerton, 2000;Candland, 1993;Catania, 1972Catania, , 1991Gardner, Gardner, & Van Cantfort, 1989;Herman, 1989;Herman & Uyeyama, 1999;Hirsh-Pasek & Golinkoff, 1996;Kako, 1999;Pepperberg, 1999aPepperberg, , 1999bPremack, 1986;Savage-Rumbaugh, Shanker, & Taylor, 1998;Terrace, 1985).…”
Section: Notementioning
confidence: 74%
“…We are therefore inclined to believe that it is arbitrary whether these tasks are called artificial grammar tasks or artificial orthographic regularity tasks. In this sense, the present research is quite different from research on language or communication in dolphins, parrots, chimpanzees, and other nonhuman animals, as well as in humans (Calvin & Bickerton, 2000;Candland, 1993;Catania, 1972Catania, , 1991Gardner, Gardner, & Van Cantfort, 1989;Herman, 1989;Herman & Uyeyama, 1999;Hirsh-Pasek & Golinkoff, 1996;Kako, 1999;Pepperberg, 1999aPepperberg, , 1999bPremack, 1986;Savage-Rumbaugh, Shanker, & Taylor, 1998;Terrace, 1985).…”
Section: Notementioning
confidence: 74%
“…Both Pepperberg (1999) and Herman and Uyeyama (1999) appear to find this suggestion attractive. Pepperberg proposes that, because parrots live in complex social systems not unlike those of primates, their "cognitive abilities, whether or not syntactically expressed, should match those of nonhuman primates" (p. 17).…”
Section: Brains Social Complexity and Convergent Evolutionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…I encourage Pepperberg (1999) and Shanker et al (1999) to take the position advocated by Herman and Uyeyama (1999), who state that "the animal language work can help us to identify with more surety those processes in humans that may derive from general cognitive structures rather than from language-specific structures" (p. 22). By con-ducting crucial experiments informed by prior research on human language acquisition, we can learn more about the extent to which language is built upon other cognitive capacities and about the power of natural selection to produce similar capacities in differently organized brains.…”
Section: The Significance Of This Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Ford,1989; (k) Sayigh et al, 1990;Ford, 1991;Barrett-Lennard, 2000. (Herman et al, , 1993Herman and Uyeyama, 1999;Pack and Herman, 2004;Tschudin et al, 2001); suggesting that they possess mechanisms for shared attention that need not be developed through formal training. They can interpret both static and dynamic gaze almost flawlessly, even in novel situations in the first set of trials (Herman, 2004), eliminating the possibility that they used an arbitrary stimulus response.…”
Section: Ecologymentioning
confidence: 99%