2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.04.012
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The development of linguistic prediction: Predictions of sound and meaning in 2- to 5-year-olds

Abstract: Language processing in adults is facilitated by an expert ability to generate detailed predictions about upcoming words. This may seem like an acquired skill, but some models of language acquisition assume that the ability to predict is a prerequisite for learning. This raises a question: Do children learn to predict, or do they predict to learn? We tested whether children, like adults, can generate expectations about not just the meanings of upcoming words but also their sounds, which would be critical for us… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
27
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2
1

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
0
27
0
Order By: Relevance
“…With that said, recent years have seen multiple publications describing weaker prediction or prediction adaptation the younger the participants (Barrault, Havron, Dautriche, Babineau, de Carvalho, Christophe, in preparation ; Beretti et al, 2020; Gambi, Gorrie, Pickering, & Rabagliati, 2018; Havron et al, 2019). Furthermore, recent studies have failed to replicate high‐profile adult studies showing fine‐grained syntactic adaptation (Harrington Stack et al, 2018; Liu, Burchill, Tanenhaus, & Jaeger, 2017).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…With that said, recent years have seen multiple publications describing weaker prediction or prediction adaptation the younger the participants (Barrault, Havron, Dautriche, Babineau, de Carvalho, Christophe, in preparation ; Beretti et al, 2020; Gambi, Gorrie, Pickering, & Rabagliati, 2018; Havron et al, 2019). Furthermore, recent studies have failed to replicate high‐profile adult studies showing fine‐grained syntactic adaptation (Harrington Stack et al, 2018; Liu, Burchill, Tanenhaus, & Jaeger, 2017).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We see in this study and others (e.g., Moerk, 1972; Read et al, 2014) that children take the pause as an opportunity to guess aloud, but future work will test if children use pre-target pauses within story reading to make silent predictions as well by tracking their anticipatory looking patterns. We already know that even without pauses children in this age range are able to rapidly anticipate words at the ends of sentences using many available types of constraining cues such as semantics (e.g., Borovsky, Elman, & Fernald, 2012) and prosody (e.g., Gambi, Gorrie, Pickering, & Rabagliati, 2018), and so given extra time and a consistent pattern of introducing attention-getting novel monster names at the end of each stanza in this book, why wouldn’t children be in a state of anticipation during a pre-target pause in this study?…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For recent reviews, including critical discussion of proposed mechanisms and limitations of prediction in L1 processing, the reader is referred to Huettig (2015) and Pickering and Gambi (2018). Prediction of upcoming words based on lexical-semantic and morphosyntactic cues has also been shown in children as young as two years old (e.g., Lew-Williams & Fernald, 2007;Mani & Huettig, 2012; but see Gambi et al, 2018, for later emergence of form-related prediction), with evidence for increasing engagement in predictive processing with increasing age and vocabulary knowledge (Fernald et al, 2008;Borovsky et al, 2012). This evidence suggests that prediction is part of human language behavior from early on in development, and that 'language prediction skills' are honed in tandem with language development more generally.…”
Section: Prediction Among Language Users Other Than Native-speaking College Studentsmentioning
confidence: 99%